Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2010 Jan 13;30(2):703–713. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4214-09.2010

Early-Life Experience Reduces Excitation to Stress-Responsive Hypothalamic Neurons and Reprograms the Expression of Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone

Aniko Korosi 1, Marya Shanabrough 2, Shawn McClelland 1, Zhong-Wu Liu 3, Erzsebet Borok 2, Xiao-Bing Gao 3, Tamas L Horvath 2,3,4, Tallie Z Baram 1,
PMCID: PMC2822406  NIHMSID: NIHMS172338  PMID: 20071535

Abstract

Increased sensory input from maternal care attenuates neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress long term and results in a lifelong phenotype of resilience to depression and improved cognitive function. Whereas the mechanisms of this clinically important effect remain unclear, the early, persistent suppression of the expression of the stress neurohormone corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in hypothalamic neurons has been implicated as a key aspect of this experience-induced neuroplasticity. Here, we tested whether the innervation of hypothalamic CRH neurons of rat pups that received augmented maternal care was altered in a manner that might promote the suppression of CRH expression and studied the cellular mechanisms underlying this suppression. We found that the number of excitatory synapses and the frequency of miniature excitatory synaptic currents onto CRH neurons were reduced in “care-augmented” rats compared with controls, as were the levels of the glutamate vesicular transporter vGlut2. In contrast, analogous parameters of inhibitory synapses were unchanged. Levels of the transcriptional repressor neuron-restrictive silencer factor (NRSF), which negatively regulates Crh gene transcription, were markedly elevated in care-augmented rats, and chromatin immunoprecipitation demonstrated that this repressor was bound to a cognate element (neuron-restrictive silencing element) on the Crh gene. Whereas the reduced excitatory innervation of CRH-expressing neurons dissipated by adulthood, increased NRSF levels and repression of CRH expression persisted, suggesting that augmented early-life experience reprograms Crh gene expression via mechanisms involving transcriptional repression by NRSF.

Introduction

Early-life experience induces persistent neuroplasticity of the neuroendocrine stress system, with implications for emotional and cognitive function (Levine, 1967). This plasticity, characterized by reduced stress responses (Plotsky and Meaney, 1993; Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001a), increased resilience to depressive-like behavior (Meaney et al., 1991), and improved learning and memory (Liu et al., 2000; Fenoglio et al., 2005), can be induced by brief daily separation of rat pups from the dam during the first weeks of life, which promotes maternal-derived sensory input (Liu et al., 1997; Fenoglio et al., 2006).

At a molecular level, adult rats experiencing augmented maternal sensory input have altered basal expression of key neuronal genes involved in regulating neuroendocrine and behavioral stress responses (Joëls and Baram, 2009). These include reduced expression of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus neurons, attenuated hormonal stress response, and increased expression of glucocorticoid receptor in hippocampal neurons (Plotsky and Meaney, 1993; Liu et al., 1997; Fenoglio et al., 2005). Experience-induced reduction in CRH expression was apparent already by postnatal day 9 (P9), preceding both the diminished hormonal stress responses and the increased hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression (Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001a). These observations, together with the fact that the “experience-augmented” phenotype could be reproduced by reducing interactions of CRH with its receptor during development (Fenoglio et al., 2005), suggested that reduced CRH expression was an early, key element of the process leading from enhanced maternal sensory input to the enduring phenotype described above.

What is the nature of signals, derived from maternal sensory experience, that reaches CRH neurons? The CRH neuron is a component of a neuronal network activated by maternal care (Fenoglio et al., 2006). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that augmented early-life experience alters excitatory and/or inhibitory synaptic input onto CRH neurons and that this alteration, in turn, triggers molecular machinery that enduringly reduces CRH expression.

Synaptic innervation of individual (including hypothalamic) neurons has recently emerged as dynamic and modulated by experience (Verkuyl et al., 2004; Horvath, 2005). For CRH neurons, the majority of input is mediated by GABAergic and glutamatergic synapses (Boudaba et al., 1997; Miklos and Kovacs, 2002; Ziegler et al., 2005), via GABAA (Cullinan, 2000) and glutamate receptors (Aubry et al., 1996; Kiss et al., 1996; Di et al., 2003). GABA and glutamate are transported into presynaptic vesicles by vesicular transporters, vGat (McIntire et al., 1997) and vGlut (Herzog et al., 2001), respectively, so that these transporters provide specific markers for inhibitory and excitatory afferent synaptic contacts onto CRH neurons. Here, we combined quantification of these markers, using Western blot, quantitative confocal and electron microscopy, and electrophysiology, to examine the effects of early-life experience on excitatory and inhibitory synapses abutting CRH neurons. We then examined the molecular machinery underlying the persistent suppression of CRH expression, testing whether augmented early-life experience increased expression of the repressor neuron-restrictive silencer factor (NRSF) (Mori et al., 1992; Palm et al., 1998), because of the presence of a functional binding site for this repressor within the Crh gene (Seth and Majzoub, 2001).

Materials and Methods

Animals.

Thirty-two timed-pregnant Sprague Dawley rats were housed in an uncrowded, quiet animal facility room on a 12 h light/dark cycle and were provided with food and water ad libitum. Parturition was checked daily, and the day of birth was considered postnatal day P0. On P2, all pups were briefly gathered, and 10 pups were assigned at random to each dam. They experienced one of the following early-life rearing conditions: (1) brief separation from the dam (augmented care, or “augmented experience”), which took place daily from P2 to P8; or (2) undisturbed controls that remained in cages that were not touched between P2 and P8. Rats were either killed on P9, between 8:00 A.M. and 12:00 P.M., or maintained under standard animal facility rearing conditions, weaned on P21, and grown three to four per cage into adulthood. All experiments were approved by the University Animal Care Committee and conformed to National Institutes of Health (NIH) guidelines.

Augmentation of maternal care via brief daily separation and observation of maternal care.

Cages were brought into the laboratory daily at 8:30 A.M. The dam and the pups were placed into separate bedded cages (pups were kept euthermic via a heating pad located underneath the cage). After 15 min, pups were placed back into their home cage, followed by the dam, and returned to the vivarium. Undisturbed litters remained in the vivarium from P2 to P9. For all experimental groups, cage change did not occur during this time. After pups and the dam were returned to the vivarium, maternal behavior (including licking and grooming) was observed using a protocol based on the work of Liu et al. (1997) as used by Fenoglio et al. (2006). Maternal behavior was also determined in undisturbed litters. Each maternal observation session consisted of 10 epochs of 3 min each. Within each epoch, the duration of licking and grooming of pups was recorded during the first minutes. The total amount of time spent licking and grooming was scored per session, added for all the sessions, and multiplied by three to provide the total amount of maternal care during the 30 min session.

In situ hybridization for CRH mRNA.

Rats were rapidly decapitated at P9 (n = 5 for control, n = 6 for experience-augmented pups) and between P45 and P60 (n = 4 per group), and the brains were quickly dissected and frozen in dry ice. Brains were sectioned at 20 μm using a cryostat, collected onto gelatin-coated slides, and stored at −80°C. In situ hybridization (ISH) histochemistry was conducted as described previously using deoxy-oligonucleotide probes (Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001b). The probe was labeled with 35S using routine terminal deoxynucleotide transferase methodology. Briefly, sections were brought to room temperature, air dried, and fixed in fresh 4% buffered paraformaldehyde (PFA) for 20 min, followed by dehydration and rehydration through graded ethanols. Sections were exposed to 0.25% acetic anhydride and 0.1 m triethanolamine, pH 8, for 8 min and dehydrated through graded ethanols. Prehybridization and hybridization steps were performed in a humidified chamber at 42°C in a solution of 50% formamide, 5× SET (750 mm NaCl, 150 mm Tris, 12.5 mm EDTA), 0.2% SDS, 5× Denhart's solution, 0.5 mg/ml salmon sperm DNA, 0.25 mg/ml yeast tRNA, 100 mm dithiothreitol, and 10% dextran sulfate. After a 1 h prehybridization, sections were hybridized overnight with 0.25 × 106 cpm of labeled probe. After hybridization, sections underwent serial washes at 42°C, most stringently at 0.3× SSC for 30 min at room temperature. Sections were then dehydrated through increasing ethanol concentrations, air dried, and apposed to film (Kodak BioMax MR Film; Eastman Kodak) for 5–10 d.

The ISH signal was analyzed on scanned, digitized paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) images of sections at coronal levels corresponding at 3.8–3.5 mm anterior to bregma. The Image Tool software program (University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio, TX) was used (Fenoglio et al., 2006) after determining the linear range of optical densities (ODs) using 14C standards. Values were only used if they fell in the linear range and were corrected for background by subtracting the OD of signal over the thalamus. The OD from two optimal sections was averaged to generate an expression value for each PVN, which is expressed in nanocuries per gram. These values were then used to calculate group means ± SEM.

Immunocytochemistry studies.

At P9 or at P45, rats were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused through the ascending aorta with 0.9% saline solution, followed by freshly prepared, cold 4% PFA in 0.1 m sodium phosphate buffer (PB), pH 7.4, cryoprotected in 15 and 30% sucrose/PB solution, and stored at −80°C. Brains were sectioned at 30 μm using a cryostat, and sections were collected as four series with 90 μm intervals between sections in tissue-culture wells containing 0.1 m PB. Every fourth matched section was subjected to immunocytochemistry (ICC). For each experiment, sections of experience-augmented and control rat brains were processed concurrently in parallel wells.

For single-labeling CRH ICC, free-floating sections of experience-augmented (n = 5) and control (n = 5) rats were subjected to standard avidin–biotin complex (ABC) methods (Fenoglio et al., 2006). Briefly, after several washes with 0.01 m PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100, pH 7.4 (PBS-T), sections were treated for 30 min in 0.3% H2O2 in PBS, followed by blockade of nonspecific sites with 1% bovine serum albumin (BSA) in PBS for 30 min and incubation for 48 h at 4°C with anti-CRH (1:40,000; gift from Dr. W. W. Vale, Salk Institute, La Jolla, CA) in PBS. After three 5 min washes in PBS-T, sections were incubated in biotinylated rabbit anti-goat IgG (1:200; Vector Laboratories) in PBS containing 1% BSA for 2 h at room temperature. After washing in PBS-T (three times for 5 min), sections were incubated in ABC solution (1:100; Vector Laboratories) for 2 h at room temperature. Sections were then rinsed again in PBS (three times for 5 min). The reaction product was visualized by incubating sections for 8–10 min in 0.04% 3,3-diaminobenzidine (DAB) containing 0.01% H2O2.

All counts were performed without knowledge of experimental group (blindly). CRH-immunopositive cells were visualized using a Nikon E400 microscope and counted in anatomically matched sections using systematic sampling methods. Briefly, CRH-immunopositive cells were counted in four sections that were 90 μm apart per animal. We obviated double counting by focusing throughout the 30 μm section and including a cell only at the level at which it had a clear complete nuclear profile. Because the diameter of the CRH-positive cell nucleus was considerably smaller than the thickness of each cryostat section, each cell was counted once only. The PVN and central nucleus of the amygdala (ACe) were sampled at coronal levels 3.8–3.5 mm anterior to bregma, and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BnST) was sampled at levels corresponding to 5.0–4.7 mm anterior to bregma. The intensity of CRH immunoreactivity was analyzed on photomicrographs taken through a digital camera (Spot Digital camera, RT color V3.0; Diagnostics Instruments) with a standardized light source and standardized exposure. Analyses were performed using ImageJ (version 1.41; NIH, Bethesda, MD). Densities are expressed in OD units after correcting for background by subtracting the density of the immunoreactive signal over the anterior commissure. Numbers of CRH-immunopositive neurons and CRH OD from four sections were averaged. The numbers of cells per section and OD per section were used to calculate group means ± SEM.

For double-labeling ICC of CRH and vGlut2 (the predominant vGlut isoform in the hypothalamus) (Herzog et al., 2001), sections of experience-augmented and control rats (n = 3 per group) were washed three times for 10 min in 0.01 m PBS and treated with 1% Triton X-100 in 0.01 m PBS for 30 min. Sections were placed in 2% normal goat serum and incubated for 48 h at 4°C in a mixture of rabbit anti-CRH serum (1:10,000) and guinea pig anti-VGlut2 serum (1:10,000; AB5907; Millipore). After three 10 min washes in 0.01 m PBS, sections were incubated in a secondary antiserum mixture (1:400; goat anti-rabbit IgG 568 and goat anti-guinea pig IgG Alexa Fluor 488; Invitrogen).

The number of vGlut2 boutons contacting PVN–CRH neurons was assessed by confocal laser-scanning microscopic analysis (n = 66 cells for control, n = 76 cells for experience-augmented rats). Images of the parvocellular division of the PVN (at coronal levels 3.8–3.5 mm anterior to bregma) were collected using an LSM510 confocal scanning system (Zeiss). Five 2-μm-thick optical sections were collected along the z-axis throughout the thickness of the whole neuron. Images were acquired with an excitation wavelength of 488 nm (green) and 543 nm (red) with a 40× oil objective using the minimum pinhole size. Images were imported as TIFF files at the resolution of 1024 × 1024 pixels. vGlut2 boutons contacting PVN–CRH neuron were counted through the complete z-axis in each optical section and averaged (vGlut2 boutons/optical section/CRH cell). A vGlut2 bouton was considered to be apposed to the CRH cell body only when there was no visible space between the CRH cell membrane and the bouton. In addition, cell size was measured in the central optical section for each CRH neuron using LSM Image browser software (version 4.2.0.121; Zeiss). Analyzed cells were selected based on the following criteria: (1) CRH immunopositive; (2) fully visible soma within the Z-stack; and (3) a clearly identifiable nucleus. After all of the quantitative analyses were completed, images used for illustration were optimized for brightness and contrast using Adobe Photoshop 7.0.

Western blot.

Samples consisted of micropunched PVN (Palkovits, 1973) or dissected thalamus from an individual rat. Rats were rapidly decapitated at P9 (n = 12 per group) and at P45 (n = 5 per group), and the brains were quickly dissected and frozen on powdered dry ice. The PVN was punched out (21 gauge needle) from consecutive 150-μm-thick cryostat sections. Tissue was homogenized in 1.5 ml Eppendorf tubes in ice-cold 0.32 m sucrose and 0.1 m Tris-HCl, pH 7.4, containing a protease inhibitor mixture (Complete; Roche; diluted according to the manufacturer's instructions). Protein concentration was determined (Bio-Rad), and equal amounts of protein were diluted in Laemmli buffer, separated by SDS-PAGE, and visualized using the enhanced chemiluminescence (ECL)-Plus kit (GE Healthcare) as described previously (Brewster et al., 2005). Briefly, 2 μg of protein extracts were separated on a 10% SDS-PAGE and transferred to Hybond-P polyvinyl difluoride membranes (GE Healthcare). Membranes were blocked with 10% nonfat milk in 1× PBS overnight at 4°C and probed with guinea pig anti-vGlut2 (1:800,000; AB5907; Millipore), rabbit anti-vGat (1:150,000; generous gift from Dr. R. Edwards, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA), or rabbit anti-NRSF (1:20,000; H-290-sc25398; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) in each case combined with rabbit anti-actin (1:60,000; Sigma). After washes in PBS-T (three times for 5 min), membranes were incubated with secondary antibodies made in donkey, conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (1:10,000) in 5% nonfat milk in PBS for 1 h at room temperature. Membranes were washed in PBS-T (three times for 5 min) and incubated with ECL-Plus. Immunoreactive bands were visualized by apposing membranes to Hyperfilm ECL (GE Healthcare). PVN and thalamic extracts of individual rats of different groups were run concurrently on the same gel. Specificity of signal was examined by excluding the primary antibodies in the presence of the secondary antibodies. These treatments resulted in no immunoreactive vGlut2, vGat, or NRSF bands.

Western blot data acquisition and analysis were accomplished by measuring the OD of immunoreactive bands from the ECL films using ImageTool. Several durations of exposure were performed to ascertain that ODs were in a linear range. The OD of vGlut2, vGat, and NRSF bands was normalized to β-actin of the same sample. Individual ODs were normalized to the mean value of the control OD and used to generate mean values per group ± SEM.

Electron microscopic immunostaining.

P9 rats were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused through the ascending aorta with 0.9% saline solution, followed by freshly prepared, cold 4% PFA; 15% picric acid; and 0.08% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 m PB, pH 7.4. Brains were removed and postfixed in 4% PFA overnight and stored in 0.1 m PB. Brains were sectioned at 30 μm using a Vibratome (Lancer), and sections were collected in multiwell plates containing 0.1 m PB. For CRH ICC, free-floating sections of experience-augmented (n = 5) and control (n = 5) rats were processed according to standard ABC methods with minor adaptations for the electron microscopic staining. Specifically, sections were treated for 30 min in 0.3% H2O2 in PBS. Sections were incubated for 1 h in freezing solution (0.1 m PB, 25% sucrose, and 10% glycerol) followed by freeze/thaw in liquid nitrogen repeated three times. Then sections were washed several times with 0.01 m PBS, followed by incubation in 1% BSA in PBS for 30 min to block nonspecific staining. After a 10 min rinse in PBS, sections were incubated for 48 h at 4°C with anti-CRH (1:40,000) in PBS containing 1% BSA and washed three times in PBS-T for 5 min each. Subsequently, sections were incubated in biotinylated rabbit anti-goat IgG (1:200; Vector Laboratories) in PBS for 2 h at room temperature. After washing in PBS (three times for 5 min), sections were incubated in ABC solution (1:100; Vector Laboratories) for 2 h at room temperature. Sections were then rinsed again in PBS (three times for 5 min). The reaction product was visualized by incubating sections for 8–10 min in 0.04% DAB containing 0.01% H2O2. Sections were then osmicated (1% OsO4 in PB) for 30 min, dehydrated through increasing ethanol concentrations (using 1% uranyl acetate in 70% ethanol for 30 min), and flat embedded in Durcupan between liquid release-coated slides and coverslips (Electron Microscopy Sciences) followed by capsule embedding. Blocks were trimmed, and ribbons of serial ultrathin sections were cut (Leica Ultracut E), collected on Formvar-coated single slot grids, and examined under an FEI Biotwin electron microscope.

Semiquantitative analysis.

The quantitative and qualitative analysis of synapse number was performed in a blinded manner. To obtain a complementary measure of an axosomatic synaptic number, unbiased for possible changes in synaptic size, the dissector technique was used. On consecutive 90-nm-thick sections, we determined the average projected height of the synapses and used about 30% of this value as the distance between the dissectors. On the basis of this calculation, the number of axosomatic synapses was counted in two consecutive serial sections about 270 nm apart (termed “reference” and “look-up” sections) of 10 CRH-immunolabeled perikarya profiles in each animal. Synapse characterization was performed at a magnification of 20,000. Symmetric and asymmetric synapses were counted on all selected neurons only if the presynaptic and/or postsynaptic membrane specializations were seen and synaptic vesicles were present in the presynaptic bouton. Synapses with neither clearly symmetric nor asymmetric membrane specializations were excluded from the assessment. The plasma membranes of selected cells were outlined on photomicrographs, and their length was measured with the help of a cartographic wheel. Plasma membrane length values measured in the individual animals were added, and the total length was corrected to the magnification applied. Synaptic densities were evaluated according to the formula NV = Q−/Vdis, where NV is the number per volume, Q− is the number of synapses present in the reference section that disappeared in the look-up section, and Vdis is the dissector volume (volume of reference), the area of the perikarya profile multiplied by the distance between the upper faces of the reference and look-up sections (i.e., the data are expressed as numbers of synaptic contacts per unit volume of perikaryon). Section thickness was determined using the Small's minimal fold method. The synaptic counts were expressed as numbers of synapses on a 100 μm membrane length unit.

Electrophysiology.

Whole-cell recordings were made from presumed CRH neurons from the parvocellular paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus. Rats were rapidly decapitated at P9 (n = 8 rats per group), and at P30 (n = 3 rats per group), brains were quickly dissected and 300 μm hypothalamic slices were cut using a vibratome. Hypothalamic slices were maintained at 33°C and perfused with artificial CSF (ACSF). The bath solution (ACSF) consisted of (in mm) 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 1.23 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, and 10 glucose, pH 7.4, with NaOH and was continuously bubbled with 5% CO2 and 95% O2. The patch pipettes were made of borosilicate glass (World Precision Instruments) with a Sutter micropipette puller (PP-97). The tip resistance of the recording pipettes was 4–6 MΩ after filling with a pipette solution containing 145 mm KMeSO4, 1 mm MgCl2, 10 mm HEPES, 1.1 mm EGTA, 2 mm Mg-ATP, and 0.5 mm Na2-GTP, pH 7.3, with KOH and 0.1% Lucifer yellow (LY). After a gigaohm seal and a whole-cell access were achieved, the series resistance was between 20 and 40 MΩ and partially compensated by the amplifier. Both input resistance and series resistance were monitored throughout the experiments. Miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) and miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) were recorded under voltage clamp in the presence of tetrodotoxin (TTX) and bicuculline with a pipette solution containing KMeSO4 (for mEPSCs) or TTX and CNQX plus AP-5 with a pipette solution containing KCl (for mIPSCs) with a multiclamp 700A amplifier (Molecular Devices). The use of KCl significantly increases the conductance of GABAA receptors and makes recording mIPSCs easier. PVN cells were held at −60 mV. Detection of mEPSC and mIPSC events were performed off-line with the software Axograph 4.9 (Molecular Devices). The frequency and amplitude of mEPSCs (or mIPSCs) were generated after detection of mEPSC events, as described previously (Gao and van den Pol, 2001). Putative CRH neurosecretory parvocellular PVN neurons were identified on the basis of their visualized position within the PVN and based on the shape of the neuron.

Immunocytochemical identification of CRH neurons.

After recording with electrodes containing LY in the pipette solution, slices were postfixed overnight in 4% PFA at 4°C overnight, washed in 0.1 m PB, embedded in agar, and sliced with a vibratome to 50-μm-thick sections. After washes in PBS-T, sections were incubated with rabbit anti-CRH (1:10,000) in 1% BSA in PBS-T for 48 h at 4°C. After three 5 min washes in PBS-T, goat anti-rabbit antibody (Alexa Fluor 568; 1:400) in 1% BSA in PBS-T was applied for 3 h to label CRH neurons. Sections were then sealed on glass slides with Vectashield to avoid bleaching and examined with a fluorescent microscope (Zeiss Axiophot microscope). The overlap of LY images with Alexa Fluor 568 staining was taken as identification of CRH neurons; however, leakage of cell contents including the LY in the slices impeded positive identification of the recorded neurons. Therefore, we considered for analysis only recorded neurons residing in the CRH-rich, dorsomedial parvocellular field of the PVN.

Chromatin immunoprecipitation.

Chromatin immunoprecipitation (ChIP) assay was based on a fast ChIP protocol (Nelson et al., 2006). Brains were dissected from P12 rats, and hippocampi were isolated, frozen in tubes on dry ice, and stored at −80°C until use. All ChIP buffer solutions contained protease inhibitor complex (Roche). Cross-linking was achieved by adding 1 ml of 1% formaldehyde in PBS, dicing the tissue with scissors, and incubating at room temperature for 20 min. Samples were spun 5 min at 800 × g at 4°C, and the supernatant was discarded. Pellets were washed twice with 1 ml of 0.125 m glycine in PBS and spun 5 min at 800 × g, and the supernatant was discarded. Next, tissue was homogenized in 1 ml of hypotonic buffer (10 mm KCl, 10 mm Tris, pH 8, and 1.5 mm MgCl2) using a Dounce homogenizer with an A pestle. Samples were then incubated on ice for 15 min, allowing the cells to swell, followed by the addition of 100 μl of 10% NP-40 and spinning of samples 1 min at 16,000 × g. The supernatant was discarded, and 360 μl of radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) lysis buffer (50 mm Tris, 150 mm NaCl, 1% NP-40, 0.1% SDS, 5 mm EDTA, and 1 mm EGTA) was added to the remaining nuclear pellet. Samples were then sonicated for 10 min on a high setting (30 s on/30 s off) using a Bioruptor sonicator (Diagenode). Samples were then spun 5 min at 16,000 × g, and the top 350 μl of the supernatant was added to 100 μl of 50% protein G-agarose bead slurry blocked with salmon sperm DNA (Millipore) and 1ml of RIPA buffer. Samples were incubated on a rotator at 4°C for 1 h, spun at 3300 × g for 1 min, and transferred to new tubes as ChIP input samples. From ChIP input tubes, 425 μl samples were aliquoted to new tubes for incubation with 2.5 μg of NRSF antibodies (H290; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) or IgG. Samples were incubated on a rotator at 4°C overnight and spun at 16,000 × g for 10 min, and the top 360 μl of the supernatant was transferred to a tube containing 30 μl of 50% protein G-agarose beads. Samples were incubated on the rotator at 4°C for 1 h and spun at 3300 × g for 1 min. The supernatant was discarded, and the beads were washed five times by adding 1 ml of RIPA buffer, incubating on the rotator at 4°C for 5 min, spinning at 3300 × g, and aspirating the supernatant. A total of 65 μl of 20% Chelex (Bio-Rad) in water was added to 20 μl aliquots of the original ChIP input samples and to each pellet. The tubes were briefly mixed with a Vortex mixer and incubated in boiling water for 10 min. The samples were allowed to cool and were spun at 16,000 × g for 2 min. The top 35 μl of supernatant containing immunoprecipitated or input DNA was transferred to new tubes for analysis.

PCR on recovered DNA was performed using primers directed at the Crh gene (forward, AGTTTGGGGAAGACTTAGGAAGAG; reverse, CTATCCGACAGACACAGACAAGAC) or the β actin gene (forward, GACTACCTCATGAAGATCCTGACC; reverse, GAGACTACAACTTACCCAGGAAGG). Gotaq green (Promega) polymerase mix was used with 10 μm of each primer and 1–4 μl of DNA template per 20 μl reaction. ChIP input DNA samples were used to adjust the amount of DNA used in each PCR to equivalent levels. After 30 cycles of PCR, products were run through gel electrophoresis along with samples of a quantitative DNA ladder (Bioline Hyperladder I). Ethidium bromide and UV light was used to visualize the DNA bands, and images were captured. Image analysis was performed on blinded samples using ImageJ software (NIH) to produce a standard curve from the quantitative DNA ladder. Band intensities for the PCR products were quantified using the standard curve.

Statistical considerations and analyses.

All analyses were conducted without the knowledge of treatment. The significance of differences between groups was set at 0.05. Differences in the maternal behavior of control and experimental dams were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA, in which treatment and time were included as fixed effects and dam was included as a repeated (subject) effect, using SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute). To correct for potential litter effect, the significance of differences among groups were examined using a mixed-model ANOVA with treatment as a fixed effect and the dam as a random effect, using SAS.

Results

Brief daily separation of the pups from their mother increases sensory stimulation of pups by the dam

Brief (15 min) separation of pups from their mother (“handling”) has been shown to enhance maternal nurturing behaviors, such as licking and grooming, toward the pups (Brown et al., 1977; Liu et al., 1997; Fenoglio et al., 2006). In addition, our previous work found that bursts or a vigorous bout of sensory input to the pups was generated during the 30 min after the return of the pups to the dam. These findings were confirmed in the current experiments: the return of the pups to the home cage elicited a barrage of maternally derived sensory stimulation. Specifically, maternal licking and grooming was increased twofold during the first 30 min after the pups' return (controls, 304.2 ± 14.4 s vs separated dams, 614.4 ± 28.2 s; F(1,31) = 39.94; p < 0.0001) These bursts of sensory stimulation of the pups occurred daily throughout the week of the experiment (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Sensory stimulation of pups by the dam is enhanced daily after brief separation of pups from their mother. Maternal stimulation of the pups, specifically licking and grooming, was observed and quantified daily from P2 to P8 during the 30 min after the return of separated pups and dams to home cages. The duration of these sensory stimulations of the pups was twofold higher in litters that were separated compared with control dams on each day (n = 16 dams per group; repeated-measures ANOVA; F(1,31) = 39.94; p < 0.0001).

CRH levels in the hypothalamic PVN are reduced by augmented early-life experience

Hypothalamic parvocellular CRH expression at the mRNA and protein levels were examined on P9 in rats experiencing the augmented maternal care from P2 to P8, compared with controls that were raised without disturbance. CRH mRNA levels in the PVN of separated rats (experience augmented) were significantly lower than those of control pups (61 ± 6.3%; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2A), and this was observed already by P9, consistent with previous findings (Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001a; Fenoglio et al., 2006). These changes in mRNA expression were translated into changes in protein expression. As shown in Figure 2, B and C, the intensity of CRH immunoreactivity over the PVN was reduced in the experience-augmented group (experience augmented, 0.13 ± 0.04 OD units/section vs control animals, 0.16 ± 0.05 OD units/section; p < 0.05), whereas the total number of CRH-expressing neurons was unchanged (control, 71 ± 5 cells/section; experience-augmented rats, 68 ± 5 cells/section). The effects of augmented early-life experience were specific to the CRH-expressing neuronal population in the hypothalamic PVN: neither the overall OD nor the total number of CRH-immunopositive cells was altered in the ACe or the BnST. Thus, CRH immunoreactivity in ACe in P9 experience-augmented and control rats averaged 0.06 ± 0.02 and 0.06 ± 0.02 OD units/section, respectively, and the numbers of neurons expressing CRH in ACe averaged 12 ± 1 and 13 ± 1 cells/section, respectively. The BnST of control rats had an average of 0.05 ± 0.02 OD units/section and 48 ± 2 CRH-expressing neurons/section compared with 0.05 ± 0.02 OD units/section and 40.0 ± 5 cells/section in BnST from experience-augmented rats.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Augmented early-life experience leads to early onset and persistent reduction in CRH expression in parvocellular PVN at both mRNA and protein levels. A, Representative bright-field photomicrographs of coronal sections at the level of the PVN from control and experience-augmented rats. The sections were subjected to ISH for CRH mRNA. CRH mRNA expression was reduced by 52% in P9 experience-augmented rats compared with undisturbed controls. B, Bright-field photomicrographs, taken under similar viewing parameters, showing CRH immunohistochemistry in PVN of control and experience-augmented rats. C, Quantitative analysis of the numbers of CRH-ir neurons and intensity of the immunoreactivity. The changes observed for mRNA expression were translated to protein levels as apparent from the ∼20% reduction in the intensity of CRH expression in experience-augmented rats. D, Representative autoradiographs after CRH mRNA ISH of CRH mRNA signal in adult control and experience-augmented rat PVN. CRH expression in experience-augmented adult rats was 50% lower compared with controls, indicating that repressed CRH expression, found on P9, was long-lasting. E, F, Bright-field photomicrographs and quantitative analysis of CRH immunohistochemistry in adult PVN. The enduring suppression of CRH expression observed at the mRNA level was translated to the protein level as evident from the ∼21% reduction in CRH-ir cells in the experience-augmented rats. 3rd, Third ventricle. Scale bars: A, D, 500 μm; B, E, 200 μm. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

The longevity of the altered CRH expression was examined in rats experiencing augmented maternal care from P2 to P8 and killed as young adults (at P45–P60). At the mRNA level, PVN CRH expression remained lower in experience-augmented rats (50 ± 4% of control values; p < 0.001) (Fig. 2D), consistent with our previous findings (Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001a; Fenoglio et al., 2005). This reduction was evident also at the protein level (Fig. 2E,F). The number of CRH-expressing neurons was lower (58 ± 4 neurons/section) in adult experience-augmented rats compared with the control cohort (75 ± 4 neurons/section; p < 0.05), whereas the intensity of CRH immunoreactivity was not altered (control, 0.06 ± 0.02 OD units/section vs experience-augmented rats, 0.07 ± 0.02 OD units/section). Similar to P9, this reduction was specific to the PVN, because no differences in the number of CRH-expressing neurons or intensity of the immunoreactive signal were detected in the ACe (control, 0.10 ± 0.05 OD units/section and 47 ± 10 cells/section; experience-augmented rats, 0.12 ± 0.05 OD units/section and 44 ± 4 cells/section) or in the BnST (control, 0.13 ± 0.05 OD units/section and 17 ± 6 cells/section; experience-augmented rats, 0.13 ± 0.07 OD units/section and 12 ± 2 cells/section). Interestingly, the numbers of CRH cells in the ACe and BnST differed in both groups between P9 and P45, with inverse trends in cell numbers with age in the two structures. We can only speculate that this might be a result of developmental changes.

Repressed CRH expression in PVN neurons of experience-augmented rats is associated with reduction in excitatory synaptic input

The reduction in CRH expression in PVN of experience-augmented rats starts early (P9), endures for life, and is associated with lifelong reduction in hormone release in response to stress, potentially indicating altered sensitivity of the CRH neuron to excitation by stress. Therefore, we tested the possibility that augmented early-life experience leads to a reduced excitatory drive (or increased inhibition) onto CRH-expressing neurons.

We first examined whether the levels of the vesicular transporters of glutamate (vGlut2) and GABA (vGat), which are markers of presynaptic elements of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses, respectively, were influenced by augmented maternal care. In punched PVN tissue, vGlut2 was detected as a single major band (∼60 kDa) (Fremeau et al., 2004). Protein levels of vGlut2 (normalized per actin and per controls) were significantly lower in experience-augmented compared with control rats: 60.5 ± 11.4 versus 101.7 ± 12.5 OD units, respectively (p < 0.01) (Fig. 3A,C). The changes in vGlut2 were selective to the PVN and were not observed in the thalamus (controls, 100.0 ± 32.4 vs experience augmented, 92.9 ± 36.9) (Fig. 3B,C). In contrast to vGlut2, protein levels of vGat were unaffected by early-life experience (49.1 ± 6.6 vs 59.1 ± 8.7 OD units in experience-augmented and control groups) (Fig. 3D).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Levels of the marker for excitatory synapses vGlut2 are reduced in PVN from immature rats with augmented early-life experience compared with controls. A, C, Representative Western blots and quantitative analysis of vGlut2 protein expression in the PVN of P9 controls and rats with augmented early-life experience. vGlut2 levels were reduced by 40% in augmented-experience rats compared with controls. B, C, Western blots and quantification of vGlut2 in thalamic tissue of P9 control and augmented early-life experience cohorts. No differences in thalamic vGlut2 levels were seen, indicating that the reduction in vGlut2 levels was specific to the PVN. D, Western blot vGat levels in the PVN of P9 control rats compared with cohorts with augmented early-life experience. vGat levels in the PVN were not affected by this early-life experience. All groups were run concurrently on the same gels, and the OD of vGlut2- and vGat-ir bands was normalized to that of actin for each lane. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

The numbers of vGlut2 boutons contacting CRH-immunoreactive (ir) neurons in the parvocellular PVN are lower in rats experiencing augmented maternal care early in life compared with controls

To examine whether the reduction in vGlut2 protein levels in experience-augmented rat PVN represented a reduced number of glutamatergic, excitatory synapses onto CRH neurons, we identified excitatory glutamatergic boutons on CRH neurons using double immunolabeling for vGlut2 and CRH. In virtual confocal sections (2 μm), vGlut2-ir axon varicosities were frequently juxtaposed onto somata and proximal dendrites of CRH-ir neurons. The CRH-expressing neurons were located in the medial parvocellular subdivision of the PVN (Sawchenko and Swanson, 1985), and their total numbers per section did not differ among the two experimental groups (see above). The numbers of vGlut2 boutons on individual CRH-ir cells were lower in experience-augmented rats (5.4 ± 0.9 vGlut2 boutons/optical section/cell) compared with controls (8.4 ± 0.9; p < 0.05) (Fig. 4). The size of CRH neurons, as assessed by measuring cell surface was not affected by the early-life experience (perimeter: controls, 32.87 ± 0.48 μm, n = 37 cells from three rats; experience-augmented pups, 33.36 ± 0.51 μm, n = 41 cells from three rats; p > 0.3).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Fewer excitatory (vGlut2-positive) boutons contact CRH-ir neurons in parvocellular PVN of immature rats with augmented maternal experience compared with the controls. A, B, Merged confocal microscope images of sections at the level of the PVN double labeled for vGlut2 (green) and CRH (red) in control and augmented early-life experience P9 rats. vGlut2-ir boutons were frequently juxtaposed onto somata and proximal dendrites of CRH-ir neurons. The number of vGlut2 boutons contacting the soma of CRH-ir neurons (arrow) was reduced by 36% in augmented early-life experience rats compared with controls, as shown in the high-magnification confocal images. C, D, CRH immunostaining (left), vGlut2-immunopositive boutons (middle), and merged image (right). Cell sizes, assessed by measurement of the circumference of the soma of PVN CRH-expressing cells, were not different between experience-augmented and control rats. Scale bars, 10 μm.

The number of asymmetric synapses on PVN–CRH-immunolabeled cells is reduced in rats that experienced enhanced maternal care early in life versus control groups

The reduced number of vGlut2-positive glutamatergic synapses abutting CRH-expressing cells could be a result of either a diminished number of synapses or a reduced synapse size. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we examined the density and structural identity of synapses onto CRH cells using electron microscopy. After immunolabeling for CRH, symmetric (inhibitory) and asymmetric (excitatory) synapses onto CRH neurons in the parvocellular PVN were examined using stereological principles. Total synaptic density was lower in experience-augmented rats compared with control cohorts (18.9 ± 6.2 vs 34.8 ± 5.2 synapses/100 μm somatic membrane; p < 0.05) (Fig. 5). Additional analysis revealed that this reduction was attributable primarily to a drastically decreased density of asymmetric excitatory synapses (from 11.6 ± 3.7 to 3.4 ± 2.2 synapses/100 μm somatic membrane; p < 0.05) (Fig. 5). In contrast to this ∼70% reduction in asymmetric synapses, the number of symmetric inhibitory synapses were not influenced by early-life experience (experience-augmented rats, 15.5 ± 5.2 synapses/100 μm somatic membrane vs undisturbed controls, 20.9 ±2.3 synapses/100 μm somatic membrane) (Fig. 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Augmented early-life experience induces a lower number of asymmetrical putative excitatory synapses onto CRH neurons. A, Left, High-magnification electron micrographs showing asymmetric (a; excitatory) and symmetric (s; inhibitory) synaptic contacts between parvocellular CRH-immunopositive soma and unidentified axon terminals (arrows). Right, Examples of synaptic contacts between CRH neurons and unidentified axon terminals (asterisks) of experience-augmented and control P9 rats. B, Quantification of the total asymmetric and symmetric synapse density revealing 50% reduction in the total number of synapses attributable to a 70% reduction in number of asymmetric synapses onto CRH neurons of augmented early-life experience rats. Scale bars, 1 μm. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

The frequency of mEPSC events is reduced in parvocellular-presumed CRH neurons of augmented early-life experience rats versus undisturbed controls

The data above indicated that CRH-expressing neurons in the PVN of experience-augmented rats were contacted by fewer asymmetric, vGlut2-positive, excitatory synapses. However, whether these structural changes influenced functional excitatory input impinging on these CRH neurons was unclear. Therefore, afferent inputs to presumed CRH neurons were measured using whole patch-clamp recordings in acute hypothalamic slices from experience-augmented and control P9 rats. Cells from the CRH-rich dorsal parvocellular region of the PVN (Fig. 6E) were held at −60 mV in the whole-cell voltage-clamp configuration, in the presence of TTX to block all action potential-driven PSCs. This enabled us to examine the frequency of mEPSCs/mIPSCs that arise from spontaneous vesicle fusion and typically reflect the number of transmitter release sites (or the probability of release) (Regehr et al., 2001). Analysis revealed that augmented early-life experience drastically reduced the frequency of mEPSCs [17.8 ± 2.8 per minute (n = 17) compared with that in controls (44.2 ± 9.4 per minute; n = 15); p < 0.01] (Fig. 6A,B), consistent with reduced numbers of presynaptic terminals (or reduced probability of release). Whereas this reduced frequency of mPSC events is indicative of presynaptic changes, the amplitude of these currents is an indication of postsynaptic changes (e.g., receptor number, function, or both) (Regehr et al., 2001). mEPSC amplitude was modestly increased in the experience-augmented rats (−12.4 ± 0.7 pA; n = 15) when compared with the control pups (−10.5 ± 0.6 pA; n = 14; p < 0.05) (Fig. 6B). In contrast to the changes in excitatory synapses, the frequency of mIPSCs did not differ between groups (control, 47.5 ± 13.6 per minute, n = 15 vs experience-augmented rats, 52.8 ± 7.4 per minute, n = 16) (Fig. 6C,D), whereas the amplitude increased modestly (−30.7 ± 2.3 pA, n = 15 vs −47.8 ± 6.8 pA, n = 16) (Fig. 6D). Thus, the electrophysiological data supported the results obtained with quantitative biochemical methods and confocal and electron microscopy.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

The frequency of mEPSC onto parvocellular CRH neurons is diminished in slices from augmented early-life experience rats. A, Raw traces of recorded mEPSCs from control and augmented early-life experience P9 rats are presented. mEPSCs were recorded in the presence of TTX (1 μm) and bicuculline (30 μm). B, Quantification representing mEPSC frequency and amplitude from parvocellular-presumed CRH neurons. The frequency of mEPSC was 60% lower in neurons recorded from experience-augmented rats compared with controls, consistent with reduced numbers of presynaptic terminals (or reduced probability of release), accompanied by a mild increase in the amplitude of mEPSC indicative of minor postsynaptic changes. C, Raw traces of recorded mIPSCs of parvocellular neuron of control and augmented early-life experience P9 rats. mIPSCs were recorded in the presence of TTX (1 μm), CNQX (10 μm), and AP-5 (50 μm). D, Quantification representing mIPSC frequency and amplitude from parvocellular-presumed CRH neurons. The frequency of mIPSC was not affected by the augmented early-life experience, whereas amplitude of mIPSC was increased, indicative of postsynaptic changes. E, To identify the recorded neurons, they were filled with LY at the time of recording and processed for CRH immunostaining. Because of leakage of cell contents and the LY in recorded neurons, only a minority were dually labeled. Therefore, all recorded neurons considered for analysis came from the CRH-rich, dorsomedial parvocellular PVN field as shown. Scale bar, 200 μm. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05. 3rd, Third ventricle.

Reduced excitatory input is not required for the maintenance of repressed CRH expression in hypothalamic neurons

CRH expression is repressed for life by augmented early-life experience (Fig. 2). The data above raised the possibility that this repression was both initiated and maintained by reduction in excitatory input. Alternatively, repression of CRH expression after augmented early-life experience might become autonomous, so that altered innervation of the CRH neuron was not required for the maintenance of this effect. To address this question, we examined excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input onto hypothalamic CRH-expressing neurons in older rats (P30–P45). Examining the levels of vGlut2, we found that by P45, protein levels of vGlut2 were no longer significantly lower than those of the control rats [145.9 ± 19.91 vs 112.8 ± 47.86 OD units (normalized per actin); p > 0.05], consistent with normalization of excitatory synapse input onto CRH neurons of the former. In accordance with this notion, the frequency of mEPSCs and mIPSCs of presumed CRH neurons in the PVN in experience-augmented and undisturbed control P30 rats were no longer different (frequency of mEPSC: control, 193 ± 31 per minute, n = 15 vs experience-augmented rats, 293 ± 48 per minute, n = 20; frequency of mIPSC: control, 276 ± 105 per minute, n = 6 vs experience-augmented rats, 414 ± 83 per minute, n = 6; p > 0.05 for all comparisons). Together, these data indicate that whereas diminished excitatory input, resulting from reduced numbers of excitatory synapses onto CRH neurons of experience-augmented rats, might be involved in initiating the molecular machinery that represses CRH gene expression in these cells, reduced excitation was not necessary to maintain the lifelong repression of the Crh gene.

Augmented early-life experience increases the levels of the NRSF, and this enhancement is persistent

If experience-induced “rewiring” of CRH-expressing neurons is not required for maintaining the repression of the Crh gene, what might the responsible molecular mechanisms for this persistent repression be? To address this question, we evaluated the role of NRSF in regulating the Crh gene by testing the ability of NRSF to bind to the Crh gene and by measuring the effects of augmented early-life experience on levels of NRSF in hypothalamic neurons. Using ChIP followed by quantitative PCR, we found that amount of “Crh gene” DNA amplified by PCR from chromatin immunoprecipitated with antiserum to NRSF was 39.2 ± 4.7 ng, indicating binding of the repressor to the regulatory region of the gene (Fig. 7A,B). The amount of Crh gene DNA immunoprecipitated with nonimmune IgG and amplified by PCR were undetectable under the ChIP conditions we used, as was the amount of amplified PCR product from the anti-NRSF immunoprecipitated DNA of the intronic region of actin, a gene devoid of NRSE sites (Fig. 7A,B). Quantifying NRSF levels in punched PVN tissue by Western blot analysis showed that NRSF migrated as a single major band (∼160 kDa). Protein levels of NRSF (normalized per actin), on P9, were dramatically higher in experience-augmented compared with control rats (147.6 ± 23.6 vs 28.0 ± 7.5 OD NRSF/actin, respectively; p < 0.05) (Fig. 7C,E). The experience-induced upregulation of NRSF persisted long term. NRSF levels were still strikingly higher in young-adult experience-augmented rats compared with controls (655.5 ± 332.9 vs 41.6 ± 10.8 OD NRSF/actin; p < 0.05) (Fig. 7D,E). This augmentation of NRSF expression was selective to the PVN, because no difference in NRSF levels was found in the thalamus (control, 199.8 ± 47.1 OD NRSF/actin; experience-augmented rats, 195.2 ± 46.9 OD NRSF/actin) (Fig. 7F). These results indicate that upregulation of NRSF levels likely contributes to both initiation and persistence of the repressed CRH expression after augmented early-life experience.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

NRSF binds specifically to the Crh gene, and its levels are dramatically increased by augmented early-life environment. A, Schematic view of the chromatin of the Crh gene. The regulatory region of the Crh gene contains an intronic NRSE, and NRSF acts as a transcriptional repressor by binding to this element, recruiting cofactors and inducing epigenetic modification of gene expression. B, Top, Amplified Crh intron DNA immunoprecipitated with anti-NRSF, yielding ∼40 ng of DNA. This is compared with undetectable DNA products immunoprecipitated with nonimmune IgG. Bottom, As an additional control for the method, the amounts of amplified Actin intron DNA (a gene devoid of an NRSE) that immunoprecipitates with anti-NRSF or with nonimmune IgG are nil. These results indicate that NRSF specifically binds to the cognate sequence (NRSE), which is located within the Crh intron. Ab, Antibody. C, E, Representative Western blots and quantitative analyses of NRSF protein expression in the PVN of P9 controls and augmented-experience rats. In concurrently run samples from both groups, NRSF levels were strikingly (5-fold) increased in augmented-experience rats compared with controls. D, E, Western blots and quantitative analysis of NRSF protein expression in the PVN of P45 controls and augmented-experience rats. The increase in NRSF levels observed on P9 was persistent, as evident from the 12-fold increase in NRSF levels on P45. F, NRSF levels were not altered by the augmented experience in the thalamus, indicating that the effect of augmented early-life experience on NRSF expression was specific to the PVN. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

Discussion

The studies described here demonstrate that after augmented early-life experience, the following occur:(1) expression of CRH mRNA and protein in hypothalamic PVN neurons is depressed, and (2) this depression is maintained to adulthood; (3) functional glutamatergic innervation of CRH neurons in the hypothalamus is markedly reduced, and (4) this experience-induced reduction in excitatory innervation resolves by adulthood, indicating that the diminished excitatory input to the CRH neuron is not required for the maintenance of the “reprogrammed” expression levels of this gene; (5) augmented early-life experience induces a marked increase in levels of NRSF, a transcription factor negatively regulating Crh gene expression, and this repressor binds directly to the regulatory region of the gene; and (6) increased NRSF levels persist to adulthood. Together, these results support the idea that augmented early-life experience reduces excitatory input onto CRH-expressing neurons. This reduced excitation is associated with, and perhaps initiates, a cascade of molecular events, including upregulation of NRSF, which may function to repress CRH expression. Whereas excitation to the CRH neuron normalizes with age, augmented NRSF levels persist, potentially contributing to maintenance of the enduring repression of CRH expression.

Early-life experience rewires hypothalamic neurons

Our data indicate that the input organization of neurons in the developing hypothalamus is not hardwired but, rather, may be influenced by early-life experience. Combining molecular, morphological, ultrastructural, and functional approaches, we found that the number of excitatory, vGlut2-ir boutons contacting CRH neurons was diminished in experience-augmented rats. This was associated with reduced vGlut2 levels in the PVN and reduced frequency of mEPSCs, and all these parameters remained unchanged in inhibitory synapses. The decreased frequency of mEPSCs could be interpreted either as reduced presynaptic excitatory elements or as reduced release probability (Regehr et al., 2001); however, the reduction in asymmetric synapses onto CRH neurons, observed using electron microscopy, supports the former possibility (the minor but significant changes in the amplitude of the mPSCs suggest postsynaptic changes that will need further investigation). Similarly, reduced total vGlut2 levels might be a result of reduced synapse size rather than synapse number, but the confocal and electron microscopy studies clarified that the number of excitatory synapses onto identified CRH neurons was attenuated after augmented early-life experience. Together, these data provide evidence for significant reduction in excitatory input onto CRH-expressing neurons of experience-augmented rats (i.e., a rewiring of these neurons).

CRH neurons in the PVN integrate excitatory and inhibitory drives from a number of sources. Both stimulatory and inhibitory afferents from limbic and cortical areas are known to coordinate CRH expression and release (Sawchenko and Swanson, 1983), yet there is little direct innervation of CRH neurons from these regions (Herman et al., 2004). Instead, axons originating in hippocampus, septum, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex contact local (peri-PVN) or remote (BnST, preoptic area) relay GABAergic or glutamatergic neurons (Tribollet and Dreifuss, 1981; Herman et al., 2004). Monosynaptic GABAergic inputs onto CRH neurons originate in regions surrounding the PVN and the BnST and maintain a strong inhibitory tone over CRH neurons (Roland and Sawchenko, 1993; Cullinan et al., 1996). Suppression of this tonic GABAergic inhibition enables secretion of CRH in response to glutamatergic activation (Cole and Sawchenko, 2002; Bartanusz et al., 2004), and functional changes in GABAergic synapses abutting CRH neurons have been described in response to physiological challenges (Miklos and Kovacs, 2002; Verkuyl et al., 2004; Ziegler et al., 2005). Glutamatergic inputs onto CRH cells in PVN originate from other hypothalamic nuclei and from BnST and amygdala (Aubry et al., 1996; Kiss et al., 1996; Di et al., 2003; Herman et al., 2004; Wittmann et al., 2005). Indeed, our previous work suggests that augmented early-life experience may reduce such excitatory input by activating a powerful inhibitory signal to amygdala or BnST from the thalamic paraventricular nucleus (Bhatnagar and Dallman, 1998; Fenoglio et al., 2006). The ability of glutamate and GABA to trigger direct synaptic actions in presumed CRH neurons (Boudaba et al., 1996, 1997) and the fact that excitation of these neurons provokes rapid transcription of the Crh gene (Cole and Sawchenko, 2002; Foradori et al., 2007), whereas elevating brain GABA levels represses CRH expression (Tran et al., 1999; Cole and Sawchenko, 2002), support the idea that the reduced balance of excitation/inhibition found here after early-life experience leads to suppression of CRH expression.

Early-life experience reprograms expression of stress-related molecules

The consequences of early-life experience (i.e., a phenotype of reduced stress responsiveness) (Plotsky and Meaney, 1993; Avishai-Eliner et al., 2001a) and improved learning and memory (Liu et al., 2000; Fenoglio et al., 2005) is associated with lifelong increased expression of GR in hippocampus and reduced expression of CRH in PVN neurons (Plotsky and Meaney, 1993; Liu et al., 1997; Fenoglio et al., 2005; Plotsky et al., 2005). We have found previously that suppression of CRH mRNA commenced already on P9 and that reduction in CRH–CRH receptor signaling sufficed to endow immature rats with this phenotype (Fenoglio et al., 2005). Therefore, we focused here on the mechanisms by which augmented early-life experience regulated CRH expression, reprogramming this expression at lower levels. As mentioned above, we found that early-life experience reduced excitation to CRH-expressing neurons and increased expression of the transcriptional repressor NRSF. However, although the experience-induced repression of CRH expression persisted, the innervation of the CRH neurons normalized by adulthood. These findings suggest that reduced excitation might contribute to the initiation of the reprogramming of CRH expression levels but was not required for its maintenance. Rather, the cellular programs repressing expression of the Crh gene have become autonomous of the reduced excitatory input.

What might these cellular mechanisms be? The levels of the transcriptional repressor NRSF were dramatically (5- to 12-fold) elevated in PVN of experience-augmented rats, and this elevation persisted in parallel to repressed CRH expression. NRSF, originally thought to silence neuronal genes in non-neural cells, is now known to be involved in neuronal plasticity (Palm et al., 1998). NRSF binds to a 21 bp sequence (NRSE), and this sequence is found within the regulatory region (intron) of the Crh gene (Seth and Majzoub, 2001). Our data are the first to show that NRSF binds specifically to this NRSE in vivo, in the hypothalamus of the developing rat. This binding should promote recruitment of cofactors contributing to epigenetic chromatin modification (Zheng et al., 2009) and, if increased after augmented maternal care, should result in repression of transcription of the Crh gene. The involvement of epigenetic mechanisms in the programming of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis by early-life maternal care has been demonstrated at the level of the promoter region of GR (Weaver et al., 2005, 2006, 2007). The enduring enhancement of hypothalamic NRSF levels, combined with the specific binding of this repressor to the Crh gene are consistent with effects of this repressor on transcription of the Crh gene throughout life.

The relationship of reduced excitation and NRSF expression is intriguing. It has been shown that neuronal activity can influence NRSF expression; for example, abnormally increased synchronized excitation (seizures) augments NRSF expression in hippocampus (Palm et al., 1998; McClelland et al., 2008). In addition, and germane to the current study, the levels of NRSF diminish rapidly during the developmental period discussed here (Palm et al., 1998). Whereas the regulation of this developmental decline of NRSF is not fully understood, it is tempting to speculate that age-dependent increase in synaptic input might be responsible for this reduction. If so, then the decreased excitatory synaptic input onto the hypothalamic CRH cell, provoked by augmented maternal care, should result in increased NRSF levels, as found here.

In summary, early-life experience leads to transient rewiring of hypothalamic neurons and reprogramming of Crh gene expression at suppressed levels. These result in a lifelong phenotype of reduced stress responsiveness as well as improved cognitive function. Therefore, understanding the basis of this experience-related plasticity is profoundly important to human health and disease and should provide the foundation of future therapeutic interventions.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants NS28912, MH73136, DK-060711, DK-080000, and DK-070723. We thank Dr. Bart Pollux for statistical advice and Jessica Cope for assistance with the in situ hybridization.

References

  1. Aubry JM, Bartanusz V, Pagliusi S, Schulz P, Kiss JZ. Expression of ionotropic glutamate receptor subunit mRNAs by paraventricular corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) neurons. Neurosci Lett. 1996;205:95–98. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12380-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Avishai-Eliner S, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Tabachnik E, Brunson KL, Baram TZ. Down-regulation of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA) precedes early-life experience-induced changes in hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor mRNA. Endocrinology. 2001a;142:89–97. doi: 10.1210/endo.142.1.7917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Avishai-Eliner S, Gilles EE, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Bar-El Y, Baram TZ. Altered regulation of gene and protein expression of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis components in an immature rat model of chronic stress. J Neuroendocrinol. 2001b;13:799–807. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00698.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bartanusz V, Muller D, Gaillard RC, Streit P, Vutskits L, Kiss JZ. Local gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate circuit control of hypophyseotrophic corticotropin-releasing factor neuron activity in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:777–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03167.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bhatnagar S, Dallman M. Neuroanatomical basis for facilitation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to a novel stressor after chronic stress. Neuroscience. 1998;84:1025–1039. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00577-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boudaba C, Szabo K, Tasker JG. Physiological mapping of local inhibitory inputs to the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. J Neurosci. 1996;16:7151–7160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-22-07151.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boudaba C, Schrader LA, Tasker JG. Physiological evidence for local excitatory synaptic circuits in the rat hypothalamus. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:3396–3400. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.6.3396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brewster AL, Bernard JA, Gall CM, Baram TZ. Formation of heteromeric hyperpolarization-activated cyclic nucleotide-gated (HCN) channels in the hippocampus is regulated by developmental seizures. Neurobiol Dis. 2005;19:200–207. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2004.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown CP, Smotherman WP, Levine S. Interaction-induced reduction in differential maternal responsiveness: an effect of cue-reduction or behavior? Dev Psychobiol. 1977;10:273–280. doi: 10.1002/dev.420100311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cole RL, Sawchenko PE. Neurotransmitter regulation of cellular activation and neuropeptide gene expression in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. J Neurosci. 2002;22:959–969. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00959.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cullinan WE. GABA(A) receptor subunit expression within hypophysiotropic CRH neurons: a dual hybridization histochemical study. J Comp Neurol. 2000;419:344–351. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000410)419:3<344::aid-cne6>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cullinan WE, Helmreich DL, Watson SJ. Fos expression in forebrain afferents to the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus following swim stress. J Comp Neurol. 1996;368:88–99. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960422)368:1<88::AID-CNE6>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Di S, Malcher-Lopes R, Halmos KC, Tasker JG. Nongenomic glucocorticoid inhibition via endocannabinoid release in the hypothalamus: a fast feedback mechanism. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4850–4857. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-04850.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fenoglio KA, Brunson KL, Avishai-Eliner S, Stone BA, Kapadia BJ, Baram TZ. Enduring, handling-evoked enhancement of hippocampal memory function and glucocorticoid receptor expression involves activation of the corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 receptor. Endocrinology. 2005;146:4090–4096. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-1285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fenoglio KA, Chen Y, Baram TZ. Neuroplasticity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis early in life requires recurrent recruitment of stress-regulating brain regions. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2434–2442. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4080-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Foradori CD, Lund TD, Nagahara AH, Koenig JI, Handa RJ. Corticotropin-releasing hormone heterogeneous nuclear RNA (hnRNA) and immunoreactivity are induced in extrahypothalamic brain sites by kainic-acid-induced seizures and are modulated by estrogen. Brain Res. 2007;1164:44–54. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fremeau RT, Jr, Kam K, Qureshi T, Johnson J, Copenhagen DR, Storm-Mathisen J, Chaudhry FA, Nicoll RA, Edwards RH. Vesicular glutamate transporters 1 and 2 target to functionally distinct synaptic release sites. Science. 2004;304:1815–1819. doi: 10.1126/science.1097468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gao XB, van den Pol AN. Melanin concentrating hormone depresses synaptic activity of glutamate and GABA neurons from rat lateral hypothalamus. J Physiol. 2001;533:237–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0237b.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Herman JP, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H. Role of GABA and glutamate circuitry in hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical stress integration. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1018:35–45. doi: 10.1196/annals.1296.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Herzog E, Bellenchi GC, Gras C, Bernard V, Ravassard P, Bedet C, Gasnier B, Giros B, El Mestikawy S. The existence of a second vesicular glutamate transporter specifies subpopulations of glutamatergic neurons. J Neurosci. 2001;21 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-j0001.2001. RC181(1–6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Horvath TL. The hardship of obesity: a soft-wired hypothalamus. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:561–565. doi: 10.1038/nn1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Joëls M, Baram TZ. The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:459–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn2632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kiss J, Gorcs TJ, Kuhn R, Knopfel T, Csaky A, Halasz B. Distribution of metabotropic glutamate receptor 1a in the rat hypothalamus: an immunocytochemical study using monoclonal and polyclonal antibody. Acta Biol Hung. 1996;47:221–237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Levine S. Maternal and environmental influences on the adrenocortical response to stress in weanling rats. Science. 1967;156:258–260. doi: 10.1126/science.156.3772.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science. 1997;277:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Liu D, Diorio J, Day JC, Francis DD, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:799–806. doi: 10.1038/77702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. McIntire SL, Reimer RJ, Schuske K, Edwards RH, Jorgensen EM. Identification and characterization of the vesicular GABA transporter. Nature. 1997;389:870–876. doi: 10.1038/39908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Meaney MJ, Viau V, Bhatnagar S, Betito K, Iny LJ, O'Donnell D, Mitchell JB. Cellular mechanisms underlying the development and expression of individual differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress response. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 1991;39:265–274. doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(91)90072-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Miklos IH, Kovacs KJ. GABAergic innervation of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH)-secreting parvocellular neurons and its plasticity as demonstrated by quantitative immunoelectron microscopy. Neuroscience. 2002;113:581–592. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00147-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mori N, Schoenherr C, Vandenbergh DJ, Anderson DJ. A common silencer element in the SCG10 and type II Na+ channel genes binds a factor present in nonneuronal cells but not in neuronal cells. Neuron. 1992;9:45–54. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90219-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Nelson JD, Denisenko O, Bomsztyk K. Protocol for the fast chromatin immunoprecipitation (ChIP) method. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:179–185. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Palkovits M. Isolated removal of hypothalamic or other brain nuclei of the rat. Brain Res. 1973;59:449–450. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90290-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Palm K, Belluardo N, Metsis M, Timmusk T. Neuronal expression of zinc finger transcription factor REST/NRSF/XBR gene. J Neurosci. 1998;18:1280–1296. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-04-01280.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1993;18:195–200. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(93)90189-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Plotsky PM, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Caldji C, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:2192–2204. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Regehr WG, Stevens CF, Cowan WM. Physiology of synaptic transmission and short-term plasticity. In: Stevens CF, editor. Synapses. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP; 2001. pp. 135–175. [Google Scholar]
  37. Roland BL, Sawchenko PE. Local origins of some GABAergic projections to the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the hypothalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1993;332:123–143. doi: 10.1002/cne.903320109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sawchenko PE, Swanson LW. The organization of forebrain afferents to the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1983;218:121–144. doi: 10.1002/cne.902180202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sawchenko PE, Swanson LW. Localization, colocalization, and plasticity of corticotropin-releasing factor immunoreactivity in rat brain. Fed Proc. 1985;44:221–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Seth KA, Majzoub JA. Repressor element silencing transcription factor/neuron-restrictive silencing factor (REST/NRSF) can act as an enhancer as well as a repressor of corticotropin-releasing hormone gene transcription. J Biol Chem. 2001;276:13917–13923. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M007745200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tran V, Hatalski CG, Yan XX, Baram TZ. Effects of blocking GABA degradation on corticotropin-releasing hormone gene expression in selected brain regions. Epilepsia. 1999;40:1190–1197. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1999.tb00847.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Tribollet E, Dreifuss JJ. Localization of neurones projecting to the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus area of the rat: a horseradish peroxidase study. Neuroscience. 1981;6:1315–1328. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(81)90190-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Verkuyl JM, Hemby SE, Joels M. Chronic stress attenuates GABAergic inhibition and alters gene expression of parvocellular neurons in rat hypothalamus. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:1665–1673. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03568.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Weaver IC, Champagne FA, Brown SE, Dymov S, Sharma S, Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Reversal of maternal programming of stress responses in adult offspring through methyl supplementation: altering epigenetic marking later in life. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11045–11054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Weaver IC, Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Maternal care effects on the hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:3480–3485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507526103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Weaver IC, D'Alessio AC, Brown SE, Hellstrom IC, Dymov S, Sharma S, Szyf M, Meaney MJ. The transcription factor nerve growth factor-inducible protein a mediates epigenetic programming: altering epigenetic marks by immediate-early genes. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1756–1768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4164-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wittmann G, Lechan RM, Liposits Z, Fekete C. Glutamatergic innervation of corticotropin-releasing hormone- and thyrotropin-releasing hormone-synthesizing neurons in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of the rat. Brain Res. 2005;1039:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zheng D, Zhao K, Mehler MF. Profiling RE1/REST-mediated histone modifications in the human genome. Genome Biol. 2009;10:R9. doi: 10.1186/gb-2009-10-1-r9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ziegler DR, Cullinan WE, Herman JP. Organization and regulation of paraventricular nucleus glutamate signaling systems: N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. J Comp Neurol. 2005;484:43–56. doi: 10.1002/cne.20445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES