Abstract
We tested the hypothesis that levels of CSF biomarkers associated with dementia and cognitive impairment are correlated with cognitive performance in non-demented Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients. Twenty-two non-demented patients with PD underwent neuropsychological testing and lumbar puncture to collect CSF. We correlated performance scores on the Logical Memory (delayed), Category Fluency, Digit Symbol, and Trails B minus A with CSF concentrations of amyloid (A) β42, total tau (t-tau), Aβ42/t-tau, and Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF). We observed significant associations between performance on the Digit Symbol test and CSF levels of Aβ42, Aβ42/t-tau, and BDNF, and between performance on the Category Fluency (vegetable) and Aβ42/t-tau. While several of these associations were attenuated by adjusting for age, our results suggest that it may be possible to use CSF biomarkers to characterize pathophysiologic processes underlying even mild cognitive deficits in non-demented PD patients.
Keywords: Parkinson disease, cognition, dementia, Alzheimer disease
Introduction
Cognitive impairment (CI) is an increasingly recognized non-motor complication of Parkinson’s disease (PD). CI of sufficient severity to fulfill criteria for dementia occurs eventually in up to 80% of PD patients [1]. CI without dementia can occur quite early in PD, and frequently is present at the time of diagnosis [1]. Importantly, CI can dominate the clinical picture in mid to late stage disease and is a major source of morbidity for patients with PD [2].
The mechanisms underlying CI in PD are likely multifactorial and possibly convergent [3]. Recent neuropathologic studies have associated Lewy-related pathology in limbic and neocortical regions with CI, but some studies also have observed that a relatively large proportion of patients with PD and dementia (PD-D) have substantial changes of Alzheimer’s disease [4–6]. Not surprisingly, vascular brain injury likely also contributes to CI in PD [3]. A limitation of autopsy-based investigations for a convergent phenotype is that variable and often lengthy amounts of time have passed between onset of cognitive impairment and pathologic examination.
A better understanding of the biologic processes contributing to CI in PD is particularly important as new disease-modifying treatments are developed. One method to investigate this issue early in the course of PD is through biomarkers. CSF biomarkers have proven to be valuable in Alzheimer’s disease where reduction in Aβ42 levels and elevated tau and phosphorylated tau have been consistently demonstrated [7]. In fact, it appears that altered concentrations of these biomarkers, especially the ratio of Aβ42 and tau levels [8–10], can predict normal individuals at increased risk for progression to CI [7]. As yet, validated biomarkers for CI in PD have not been described, although emerging data suggest that decreased CSF Aβ42 without increased CSF tau concentrations may be associated with CI in PD [11], and other initial data suggest that CSF Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) might have utility in the diagnosis of PD [12].
In this study we examined a group of non-demented PD patients who underwent detailed neuropsychological testing and lumbar puncture. We tested the hypothesis that, even in this non-demented group of PD patients, we would observe a relationship between cognitive performance and a subset of CSF biomarkers linked to CI in AD or PD.
Methods
Subjects were recruited through the Pacific Northwest Udall Center and University of Washington Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center. The Human Subject Institutional Review Boards of the VA Puget Sound Health Care System and the University of Washington approved this study. All individuals provided informed consent and underwent evaluation that consisted of medical history, physical and neurologic examinations, laboratory tests, and neuropsychological assessment. Laboratory evaluation included complete blood count (serum electrolytes, blood urea nitrogen, creatinine, glucose, vitamin B12, and thyroid stimulating hormone); all results were within normal limits. All subjects fulfilled UK PD Society Brain Bank (UKPDSBB) clinical criteria for idiopathic PD as determined by a movement disorder specialist. [13]. A consensus panel determined the presence or absence of dementia, based on both neuropsychological testing and presence or absence of CI by informant history (Clinical Dementia Rating [14]). For this study, we focused our analysis on memory (Logical Memory II), Category Fluency (animal, vegetable), working memory/executive function (Trails B minus A), and processing speed (Digit Symbol) [15].
Research lumbar punctures (LPs) were performed as previously described [12]. Briefly, all CSF was obtained by lumbar puncture in the morning, was free of visual contamination by blood, had hemoglobin levels < 6.0 µg/ml, and was flash frozen and then stored at −80°C in polypropylene cr yovials until used. Coded samples were analyzed by individuals who did not know the corresponding clinical information. CSF samples were analyzed with a Luminex-based multianalyte profile (MAP) from Biosource (InVitrogen, CA) that included total tau (t-tau), BDNF, interleukin 8, Aβ42, β2-microglobulin, vitamin D binding protein, apolipoprotein (apo) AII, and apoE as described previously [12].
Statistical Analysis
Associations between CSF biomarker and CI measurement and between these variables and age were determined using Spearman rank correlation coefficients (r). To determine if statistically significant associations were independent of age and gender, linear regressions of CI measurement on CSF biomarker were carried out with age and gender as covariates. Regression analyses were conducted on ranked CI measurements and CSF biomarkers to be consistent with the Spearman rank coefficients described above. Due to spurious findings that can arise when using ratios, statistical testing of the association between CI measurement and Aβ42/t-tau as a covariate were carried out using Aβ42 adjusted for t-tau (i.e. using both Aβ42 and t-tau as covariates in the linear regression models of CI measurements and reporting the significance of the coefficient for Aβ42). Significance was set at p<0.05. Analyses were carried out using R 2.9.1 (www.R-project.org).
Results
Twenty-two subjects (17 male, 5 female) with PD and no dementia participated in this study. Mean Mini-Mental State Examination score (SD, range) was 28 (2, 25–30). The mean age at onset of motor symptoms was 59.2 years (11.6, 39–77). Mean Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (Part III) and Hoehn and Yahr scores at the time of neuropsychological testing were 21 (9, 7–42) and 2.5 (0.8, 1.5–4), respectively. The mean time between neuropsychological testing and LP was 0.6 years. The mean age at time of neuropsychological testing was 68.7 years (8.0, 56–81) and mean years of education was 16.5 (2.4, 12–20). Summary statistics for performance scores on the five neuropsychological tests are presented in the Table.
Table.
Correlations of Mini-Mental State Examination, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (Part III), and neuropsychological test results with CSF biomarkers
Spearman rank correlation coefficient | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Median (range) |
CSF Aβ42 |
CSF t-tau |
CSF Aβ42/ t-tau** |
CSF BDNF |
|
MMSE | 28 (25–30) | 0.13 | −0.30 | 0.32 | 0.13 |
UPDRS | 17 (7–42) | −0.25 | −0.12 | −0.19 | −0.06 |
Logical Memory II | 15 (2–34) | 0.12 | −0.24 | 0.34 | −0.07 |
Category Fluency (animal) | 17 (7–24) | 0.20 | −0.24 | 0.29 | 0.34 |
Category Fluency (vegetable) | 12 (5–20) | 0.38 | −0.36 | 0.50*† | 0.38 |
Digit Symbol | 39 (19–60) | 0.49* | −0.10 | 0.50* | 0.54*‡ |
Trails B minus A | 52 (5–250) | −0.13 | −0.01 | −0.13 | −0.23 |
.01<p<.05, Spearman rank tests
.01<p<.05 after adjusting for age in linear regression of log performance on log biomarker
.01<p<.05 after adjusting for gender in linear regression of log performance on log biomarker
Statistical inference carried out using Aβ42 and total tau as covariates in regression of test performance, and reporting significance for the Aβ42 coefficient
CSF samples were analyzed with a Luminex-based MAP as previously described [12]. Values were not elevated beyond age-matched controls (data not shown). Mean (range) levels for the markers of interest were 544.4 (222–1389) pg/ml for Aβ42, 284.6 (164– to 406) pg/ml for BDNF, and 136.6 (81–269) pg/ml for t-tau.
Neuropsychological test score correlations with Aβ42, t-tau, Aβ42/t-tau, and BDNF are presented in the Table and exemplary scatter plots of raw data are presented in the Figure. Results from all other members of our CSF MAP (not shown) did not show a significant correlation with any neuropsychological test outcome. A significant correlation was found between Aβ42 and performance on Digit Symbol (r=0.49, p=0.02), and between Aβ42/t-tau and performance on both Digit Symbol and Category Fluency (vegetable) (r=0.50, p=0.02 for both). Consistent with our hypothesis that AD biomarkers might be associated with AD-like cognitive impairment, we observed a trend for an association between lower Aβ42/t-tau and more impaired performance on delayed memory (Logical Memory II), a cognitive hallmark of AD, (r=0.34), although this latter finding did not reach significance in this relatively small sample (p=0.12). CSF BDNF concentration was significantly associated with Digit Symbol performance (r=0.54, p=0.01), but no other neuropsychological test result. Adjustment for age or gender attenuated several of the observed associations (Table). Associations remained between Aβ42/t-tau and Category Fluency (vegetable) after adjusting for age (p=0.04) and between BDNF and Digit Symbol after adjusting for gender (p=0.04); the association between BDNF and Digit Symbol was not quite significant after adjusting for age (p=0.06). This was not unexpected, given that age was negatively correlated with Digit Symbol (r=−0.46, p=0.03) and weakly negatively correlated with all other neuropsychological test results (0.06<p<0.13), except Trails B minus A where there was no correlation (p=0.4).
Figure.
Scatter plots of data from the associations observed in non-demented patients with PD shown with loess curves. (A) Aβ42 vs. Digit Symbol, (B) BDNF vs. Digit Symbol, (C) Aβ42/t-tau vs. Category Fluency (vegetable), and (D) Aβ42/t-tau vs. Digit Symbol.
Discussion
Cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with PD likely is a convergent phenotype that derives from multiple diseases that variably combine in a given individual [3]. Discrimination among these disease processes early in the course of illness will be very important to tailor treatments that suppress the processes associated with Lewy body disease, AD, or vascular disease. Furthermore, there is growing consensus that therapeutic interventions for illnesses that culminate in dementia will be most effective in pre-clinical and prodromal stages.
Here we tested the hypothesis that validated biomarkers linked to PD and AD would be associated with neuropsychological test performance in a group of non-demented PD patients. The strongest associations were observed between performance on Digit Symbol and CSF levels of Aβ42, Aβ42/t-tau, and BDNF, but not t-tau. We also observed a significant association between Aβ42/t-tau ratio and Category Fluency (vegetable), but no significant association between performance on Trails or Category Fluency (animal) with any of the CSF biomarkers. This pattern of results likely reflects, at least in part, the variance in difficulty in performing of these tasks. Performance on Digit Symbol is likely more sensitive to mild CI because it is supported by a number of component processes mediated by the frontal lobes. Notably, while motor-speed is also a determinant of Digit Symbol performance, other motor-dependent tasks (i.e., Trails) and the motor UPDRS were not correlated with CSF biomarkers. Thus, it is likely the correlations observed with Digit Symbol were less dependent on motor speed and more dependent on cognitive performance. Other investigators have also observed that Digit Symbol and Category Fluency are sensitive to early cognitive change (i.e., pre-dementia) in PD [16,17]. Examination of the co-variates of age and gender suggest that the association between Aβ42/t-tau and Digit Symbol or Category Fluency may not be independent. However, this statistical finding is difficult to interpret given the small sample size.
As expected, our results showed an association between neuropsychological test results and age. Studies in larger samples will be needed to assess further the potentially confounding influence of age and gender on the association between CSF biomarkers and cognitive performance in non-demented patients with PD, and to confirm the possible lack of association observed with tests other than Digit Symbol and Category Fluency.
Despite the small sample size, we were able to observe an association of CSF markers with cognitive test performance in non-demented PD patients. The directions of these associations were consistent with previous studies in neurodegenerative disease (i.e., lower Aβ42 and BDNF levels with poorer test performance). Also consistent with previous findings in PD-D [11], we did not find an association between CSF t-tau levels and cognitive performance in our non-demented sample. While confirmation of these findings in a larger sample is necessary, they suggest it might be possible to use CSF biomarkers to characterize pathophysiologic processes underlying even mild CI in non-demented PD patients.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs, grants from the NIH (P50AG05136 and P50NS062684, R01AG033398, R01NS057567, K01AG23640), the C-M Shaw Endowment, and the Nancy and Buster Alvord Endowment. We thank Dr. Kathleen Montine for editorial assistance.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
(Additional references added at the request of the reviewers)
- 1.Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Kragh-Sorensen P. Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease: an 8-year prospective study. Archives of Neurology. 2003;60:387–392. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Buter TC, van den Hout A, Matthews FE, Larsen JP, Brayne C, Aarsland D. Dementia and survival in Parkinson disease: a 12-year population study. Neurology. 2008;70:1017–1022. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000306632.43729.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sonnen JA, Postupna N, Larson EB, Crane PK, Rose SE, Montine KS, et al. Pathologic correlates of dementia in individuals with lewy body disease. Brain Pathol. 2010;20:654–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2009.00371.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Apaydin H, Ahlskog JE, Parisi JE, Boeve BF, Dickson DW. Parkinson disease neuropathology: later-developing dementia and loss of the levodopa response. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:102–112. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.1.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Galvin JE, Pollack J, Morris JC. Clinical phenotype of Parkinson disease dementia. Neurology. 2006;67:1605–1611. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000242630.52203.8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hurtig HI, Trojanowski JQ, Galvin J, Ewbank D, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, et al. Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 2000;54:1916–1921. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.10.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sonnen JA, Montine KS, Quinn JF, Kaye JA, Breitner JC, Montine TJ. Biomarkers for cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly people. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7:704–714. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70162-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fagan AM, Roe CM, Xiong C, Mintun MA, Morris JC, Holtzman DM. Cerebrospinal fluid tau/beta-amyloid(42) ratio as a prediction of cognitive decline in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:343–349. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.3.noc60123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Li G, Sokal I, Quinn JF, Leverenz JB, Brodey M, Schellenberg GD, et al. CSF tau/Abeta42 ratio for increased risk of mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study. Neurology. 2007;69:631–639. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000267428.62582.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mattsson N, Zetterberg H, Hansson O, Andreasen N, Parnetti L, Jonsson M, et al. CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Jama. 2009;302:385–393. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Montine TJ, Shi M, Quinn JF, Peskind ER, Craft S, Ginghina C, et al. CSF Abeta(42) and tau in Parkinson's disease with cognitive impairment. Mov Disord. 2010 doi: 10.1002/mds.23287. epub. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zhang J, Sokal I, Peskind ER, Quinn JF, Jankovic J, Kenney C, et al. CSF multianalyte profile distinguishes Alzheimer and Parkinson diseases. Am J Clin Pathol. 2008;129:526–529. doi: 10.1309/W01Y0B808EMEH12L. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;51:745–752. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.6.745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lezak MD. In: Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th edition. Howieson DB, Loring DW, Lezak MD, editors. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology. 2009;72:1121–1126. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Goldman WP, Baty JD, Buckles VD, Sahrmann S, Morris JC. Cognitive and motor functioning in Parkinson disease: subjects with and without questionable dementia. Arch Neurol. 1998;55:674–680. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.5.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]