Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013 Oct-Dec;27(4):10.1097/WAD.0b013e31827bde32. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31827bde32

Defining MCI in the Framingham Heart Study Offspring: Education vs. WRAT-based norms

Richard Evan Ahl 1, Alexa Beiser 2, Sudha Seshadri 3, Sanford Auerbach 4, Philip A Wolf 5, Rhoda Au 6
PMCID: PMC3626741  NIHMSID: NIHMS425876  PMID: 23314066

Abstract

Introduction

Psychometric definitions of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) typically use cut-off levels set at 1.5 standard deviations below age- and education-adjusted norms, assuming that the education adjustment accounts for premorbid abilities. However, non-cognitive factors impact educational attainment, potentially leading to incorrect categorization as MCI. We examined whether using an adjustment based on reading performance (Wide Range Achievement Test [WRAT] Reading) improved MCI diagnostic accuracy.

Methods

935 Framingham Offspring (mean age 72 ± 5) underwent tests of Memory, Executive Function, Abstraction, Language, and Visuospatial Function as part of a neuropsychological test battery. Domain-specific test scores were regressed onto age and WRAT score, or education, to define MCI. Survival analyses were used to relate baseline MCI to incident dementia.

Results

The two MCI definitions differed most for the lowest and highest education groups. The WRAT definition was more strongly associated with incident dementia for all five tests. MCI-level Abstraction performance was associated with incident dementia using the WRAT definition (HR = 3.20, p = .033), but not the education definition (HR = 1.19, p = .814).

Discussion

The WRAT should be considered along with the standard measure of years of education, as it may be a better surrogate marker of premorbid abilities.

Keywords: Mild cognitive impairment, premorbid abilities, neuropsychological assessment, Alzheimer's disease, longitudinal

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a transitional clinical stage between normal aging and mild dementia. Its diagnosis requires evidence suggestive of cognitive decline, by self or informant report, as well as by impaired performance on neuropsychological tests based on published norms or on changes in a given individual over time. It also requires that the cognitive decline be insufficient to cause the functional deficits required for a diagnosis of dementia.1,2 In many studies, evidence of cognitive change from a premorbid level is inferred based on low test scores during a single cognitive screening; it is not directly demonstrated by decline over multiple testing sessions.3

An elderly person with low premorbid cognitive ability, and resultant low test scores, should not be classified as having developed MCI if his scores do not represent a substantial decline from his own baseline. To avoid such misclassifications, diagnostic methods need to account for premorbid functioning. One method relies on clinical judgment, rather than strict cutoff scores, to determine the likelihood that a participant's performance on a given test represents a decline from his or her premorbid level of functioning.4,5 Information such as the participant's educational and occupational background, and scores on cognitive tests designed to measure premorbid abilities, can help shape the clinician's judgment. The most common method, however, uses age and education-based norms when determining the threshold for MCI-level performance on a given test. This threshold is often set at 1.5 SDs below the reference-group mean.1,3,613 Depending on the characteristics of the population being studied, years of education (expressed as a continuous variable), or the attainment of a specific benchmark (i.e. high school diploma), may be used to determine the `education-based norms.'

While convenient in large-scale epidemiological studies, the use of education-based norms can be problematic. Educational attainment can be influenced by many non-cognitive factors, including income, race, immigrant status, and gender, especially in the current generation of elderly persons.14,15 Moreover, quantity of education is not synonymous with quality.16 For individuals attending poorly-funded and/or segregated schools, the `years of education' measure could overestimate the amount of learning that took place. Manly et al16 and O'Bryant et al17 found this to be the case when they compared racially diverse participants' reported years of education to their scores on the Wide Range Achievement Test: Third Edition (WRAT-3) Reading subtest,18 a word identification test that has been validated as a measure of premorbid functioning. Many participants scored lower on the WRAT-3 than their years of education would have predicted. There are also cohort effects that the `years of education' measure does not capture. For instance, an average 9th grade education in 1950 was different from an average 9th grade education in 2010, in its scope and rigor; this fact complicates the application of cross-generational education based cut-points to identify sub-optimal performance.

For highly-educated individuals, or those with considerable cognitive pursuits after they completed their formal education, a score in the `normal' range defined using an education cut-point could mask a significant decline in their own premorbid abilities. In a study of highly intelligent elderly participants, Rentz et al14 found that utilizing cut-off scores based on participants' performance on the American version of the National Adult Reading Test (AMNART)19 yielded significantly more-sensitive diagnoses of progressive cognitive decline than using education-based cut-offs. For most participants, the AMNART-based thresholds for `cognitively intact' scores were much higher than the education-based ones. Thus, actual impairment was better identified among the highly intelligent when using the AMNART-based thresholds. Manly et al16 argued that scores on tests of reading performance, such as the WRAT-3 and the AMNART, measure educational experience more accurately than years of education, and thus constitute a better estimate of the skills and information required to satisfactorily complete neuropsychological tests. The use of education-based norms with a broad range of educational attainment across study samples may contribute to the widely divergent estimates of MCI's prevalence and time course in the literature. It may also partly account for the high percentage of prevalent MCI cases that “improve” or “revert to normal” at subsequent visits, at rates ranging from 10% to 50%20,21 for different subtypes of MCI as persons with randomly `low' scores regress towards the mean.

The purpose of this study is to examine how MCI case identification is differentially impacted when using education-based versus reading ability-based norms in a community-based young-old sample. The reliability and validity of MCI categorization will be examined through rates of conversion back to normal and progression to mild dementia. To our knowledge, this is the first large-scale, population-based study to use an objective test of premorbid abilities as the basis for the cut-off scores used to determine a participant's MCI status at a single cognitive assessment.

Methods

The Framingham Heart Study is a longitudinal, prospective, community-based study of risk factors for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease, with initial enrollment of 5209 Original participants in 1948. Their Offspring (n=5124) came into the study in 1971, and each participant had at least one biological parent in the first-generation FHS Original cohort or was the spouse of a participant with an Original cohort parent. Detailed information about demographics and recruitment is provided elsewhere; briefly, all participants have a structured medical history, physical examination and laboratory testing done once every 2–4 years, and since 1982 these assessments have included a Folstein Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE).22,23 Medical records from hospitalizations and office visits are requested at the time of each physical exam.

As part of a longitudinal study of brain aging, 2607 FHS Offspring participants had an initial (1999–2005) comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation, and results from the assessments of 2506 participants were used to calculate FHS-specific norms. 96 of the participants excluded from the norms were excluded due to prevalent clinical dementia, stroke, or other neurological illnesses, such as multiple sclerosis or brain tumors, that could impact cognitive test scores. For this study, participants who did not meet our minimum age criterion of at least 65 years were also excluded (n=1571). The final study sample included 935 FHS Offspring participants (mean age = 72 ± 5, 53% women) who were free of dementia and stroke at their initial assessment. The corresponding MMSE was administered within 0.9 ±1.0 years of their initial NP assessment. The median score was 29, with an interquartile range of 28 – 30. Of the 935 participants, 535 completed follow-up neuropsychological evaluations, which were conducted between August 2005 and December 2009. Testing took place at the study site as well as at participants' places of residence, including private houses and nursing homes. The study was given full IRB approval. All participants received an explanation of their role in the study, gave their consent to participate, and signed informed consent forms.

Neuropsychological Evaluation

Participants were tested using a comprehensive neuropsychological (NP) battery, which included tests of Memory (WMS: Logical Memory – Delayed (LM-D) and Visual Reproductions – Delayed (VR-D)), Attention and Executive Function (Trail Making Test A and B), Abstraction (WAIS: Similarities), Language (Boston Naming Test, 30 items), and Visuospatial Function (Hooper Visual Organization Test). Standard test administration and scoring procedures were employed and previously described.23 LM-D and VR-D scores were summed to create a combined Memory score (M). Trails A timed performance was subtracted from Trails B to compute a measure of Executive Function (TrB-TrA; EF).

Premorbid Ability Assessment

Two methods were used to assess levels of premorbid ability and educational quality. Both methods used information that was collected during the neuropsychological battery administration. The first was based on self-reported educational background. For our primary analysis, participants were divided into four groups: no high school diploma, high school diploma but no college attendance, some college attendance but no four-year degree, and four-year college degree or higher. The second method was based on Wide Range Achievement Test: Third Edition, Reading subtest scores. The WRAT:R is a test of one's ability to correctly read and pronounce words of increasing difficulty.18 Reading tests have been validated as measures of premorbid abilities in racially diverse populations of young and elderly adults.16,17,24 Importantly, one's ability to read words is mostly preserved in early stages of cognitive impairment and dementia, even as other cognitive functions decline significantly.24 This makes the WRAT a suitable “hold” test, and it has good test-retest reliability in aging individuals.25,26 For all analyses, WRAT scores were log-transformed (LWRAT) to normalize their skewed distribution.

MCI Case Identification

Two different psychometric methods were used to determine the thresholds for MCI-level performance: level of education and WRAT scores.

  • MCI-WRAT: Using the baseline cross-sectional sample of participants free of stroke, dementia and other neurological conditions in the normative NP study (n=2506,1999–2005), each NP measure was regressed onto age and LWRAT. Residuals from that regression were standardized to standard normal (Z-WRAT). For each of the cognitive variables, indicators of impairment (Z-WRAT < −1.5) were created and designated MCI; for example, MCI-WRAT-M, for memory.

  • MCI-EDUC: Using the process described above, each NP was separately regressed onto age and a 4-level education group (no HS diploma, HS diploma, some college, college graduate) instead of LWRAT, a second standardized residual (Z-EDUC) was calculated and a similar indicator of impairment was created (for example, MCI-EDUC-M).

For the purposes of this study, the MCI and non-MCI groups were determined using education or WRAT-adjusted test scores exclusively.

Incident Dementia

After their initial NP assessments, participants were monitored for symptoms of incident dementia. These methods have been described in detail previously.27 The length of follow-up ranged from 5 to 11 years, through 2010, with a mean of 6 years. If cognitive impairment or dementia was suspected, the participant's case was brought to a consensus conference that included at least one neurologist and one neuropsychologist. Dementia was diagnosed according to DSM-IV guidelines. Definite, probable and possible Alzheimer's disease (AD) diagnoses met current NINDS-ADRDA28 criteria. Survival analyses were used to compare those with and without MCI with respect to incident AD. Survival time was the date of diagnosis of dementia for cases and date of censoring for non-cases.

Results

Figure 1 shows the distribution of WRAT scores amongst the four education groups. The boxplots display the 25th and 75th percentile WRAT scores, and the whiskers display the minimum and maximum WRAT scores. There is considerable variability both between and within the groups. For instance, the WRAT score in the 75th percentile of the No HS Diploma group, 45, was higher than the 25th percentile score of the HS Diploma group, 43. The maximum possible score on the WRAT is a 57.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Distribution of WRAT Score by Education Group

Note: The Maximum WRAT score is 57.

Table 1 shows the number of participants classified as cognitively intact or MCI, based on their performance on tests of Memory (MCI-M), Executive Function (MCI-EF), Abstraction (MCI-Ab), Language (MCI-L), and Visuospatial Function (MCI-VS).1 The MCI participants were divided into three groups: those classified as MCI using both education-based and WRAT-based norms, those classified as MCI using education-based norms only, and those classified as MCI using WRAT-based norms only. The percent discordant represents the percentage of participants classified as MCI by only one of the two methods. It was calculated for each test by adding the education-only and WRAT-only groups and then dividing this sum by the total number of participants. Due to the higher discordant percentages for Memory and Abstraction and the results of the incident dementia analyses (see Table 3), additional analyses stratified these discordant calculations by the four education groups. For Memory, the discordant percentages for the No HS Diploma, HS Diploma, Some College, and College Graduate groups were, 8.2%, 2.2%, 5.3%, and 5.8%, respectively. For Abstraction, the discordant percentages for the four groups were, 6.7%, 2.5%, 4.8%, and 5.4%, respectively. Discordance between MCI defined using WRAT-based norms versus education-based norms was greatest for participants with no HS diploma and least for those with HS diplomas but no further education.

Table 1.

Numbers of Participants Classified as MCI at Their First Neuropsychological Assessment, using Education versus WRAT

Cognitively Intact: Both Methods MCI: Both Methods MCI: Education Only MCI: WRAT Only % Discordant
MCI: M 829 63 21 20 4.4
MCI: EF 854 63 9 1 1.1
MCI: Ab 839 56 21 18 4.2
MCI: L 860 33 19 23 4.5
MCI: VS 876 43 7 8 1.6

MCI: M = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Memory

MCI: EF = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Executive Function

MCI:Ab = classified as MCI based on performance on a test of Abstraction

MCI:L (Language) = classified as MCI based on performance on the BNT

MCI: VS (Visuospatial) = classified as MCI based on performance on the Hooper

Table 3.

MCI using Education versus WRAT: Association with Incident AD

Cases/n:29/835 Adjusting for 4-Level Education Group Adjusting for WRAT
HR p HR p
MCI:M 14.38 [6.61–31.31] <0.001 14.46 [6.67–31.36] <0.001
MCI: EF 4.49 [1.77–11.38] 0.002 5.57 [2.19–14.14] <0.001
MCI: Ab 1.19 [0.28–5.03] 0.814 3.20 [1.10–9.32] 0.033
MCI: L 2.25 [0.68–7.47] 0.187 2.62 [0.90–7.65] 0.078
MCI: VS 1.68 [0.40–7.11] 0.479 2.27 [0.68–7.55] 0.183

HR = Hazard Ratio

MCI: M = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Memory at the first neuropsychological assessment

MCI: EF = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Executive Function at the first neuropsychological assessment

MCI:Ab = classified as MCI based on performance on a test of Abstraction at the first neuropsychological assessment

MCI:L = classified as MCI based on performance on the BNT at the first neuropsychological assessment

MCI: VS = classified as MCI based on performance on the Hooper at the first neuropsychological assessment

Table 2 shows, for the 535 participants who also completed a follow-up neuropsychological assessment, NP 2 (average time interval between assessments = 6 years), their MCI status at the two visits, using the education and WRAT-based classification methods. The last column shows the percentage of participants whose cognitive status “improved,” or “reverted to normal,” over time: Their test scores classified them as MCI at the first assessment but did not meet performance criteria for MCI at the second assessment. Neither method was superior at minimizing the amount of participants who “revert to normal” from the first visit to the second.

Table 2.

Numbers of Participants Classified as MCI at Both Neuropsychological Assessments

Using 4-Level Education Normal for Both Tests MCI for Both Tests Normal at NP 1, MCI at NP 2 MCI at NP 1, Normal at NP 2 (Reverters) All Participants % of Participants who are Reverters
MCI: M 480 11 18 23 532 23/34=68%
MCI: EF 445 12 48 10 515 10/22=45%
MCI: Ab 484 7 18 26 535 26/33=79%
MCI: L 500 10 10 13 533 13/23=57%
MCI: VS 510 5 4 13 532 13/18=72%
Using WRAT
MCI: M 473 13 21 25 532 25/38=66%
MCI: EF 445 11 51 8 515 8/19=42%
MCI: Ab 482 5 24 24 535 24/29=83%
MCI: L 491 13 10 19 533 19/32=59%
MCI: VS 510 2 7 13 532 13/15=87%

NP 1 = the first neuropsychological assessment

NP 2 = the second neuropsychological assessment

MCI: M = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Memory

MCI: EF = classified as MCI based on performance on tests of Executive Function

MCI:Ab = classified as MCI based on performance on a test of Abstraction

MCI:L = classified as MCI based on performance on the BNT

MCI: VS = classified as MCI based on performance on the Hooper

Of the 935 participants, 835 had sufficient follow-up to allow the investigators to assess the development of subsequent dementia. 29 (3.5%) developed definite, probable or possible Alzheimer's dementia over a mean follow-up of 6 years (range = 5 – 11). Table 3 shows the hazard ratios (HR) for developing AD, comparing participants with and without MCI-level scores at their first cognitive assessment. Results are shown for each of the cognitive domains, and using the education and WRAT methods. Participants with MCI-M had a significantly increased risk of developing dementia, using both the education (HR = 14.38 p < .001) and WRAT (HR = 14.46, p < .001) methods. Participants with MCI-EF also had a significantly increased risk of developing dementia, with slightly higher HRs using the WRAT method (HR = 5.57, p <.001) than the education method (HR = 4.49, p = .002). For Abstraction, using the education method, participants with MCI-Ab did not have a significantly increased risk of developing dementia (HR = 1.19, p = .814). However, participants with MCI-Ab using the WRAT method had a significantly increased risk of developing dementia (HR = 3.20, p = .033). Participants with MCI-L and MCI-VS did not have a significantly increased risk of developing dementia, regardless of whether the WRAT or education method was used.

To further compare the utility of the education and WRAT methods in predicting dementia, additional analyses were conducted on the Abstraction test scores. To more accurately distinguish the educational attainment of participants with higher education degrees, the highest level, College Graduate, was split into two groups, College Graduate Only and Postgraduate Degree. The norms were recalculated based on five education levels, and the survival analyses were conducted again. While the HRs for MCI-level Abstraction scores were higher when using five levels (HR=1.74, p=0.369) than four (HR=1.19, p=.814), participants with MCI-level Abstraction scores using five levels of education-based norms still did not have a significantly increased risk of developing dementia.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to determine if using WRAT-based norms to define MCI yields different groups of participants than using education-based norms, and if one method demonstrates an advantage over the other. The results showed some differences, particularly for the Memory, Abstraction, and Language tests, for which 4.4%,4.2%, and 4.5% of participants, respectively, were classified as MCI by one method but not the other. Although the magnitude of this difference may seem small, when focusing just on MCI participants, rather than the entire sample, the difference is more pronounced. For instance, of the 95 participants with MCI-Ab by any method, 56 were classified as MCI according to both methods, 21 by education alone, and 18 by WRAT alone. This means that, for populations that are expected to have higher rates of MCI (for instance, oldest-old or memory clinic samples), there may be a greater discordance between the two methods. One explanation for why MCI-EF showed the least discordance is that the executive function abilities measured by the Trails B test are not strongly correlated with measures of general intelligence2931 or years or education.32 Factors other than premorbid abilities/educational exposure appear to strongly influence individual differences in Trails B performance, and neither the education nor the WRAT method fully account for such differences.

For MCI-M and MCI-Ab, the greatest levels of discordance were found for the lowest (No HS Diploma) and highest (College Graduate) levels of education. This could be attributed to a high potential for a divergence between premorbid abilities and actual educational attainment in the case of the No HS Diploma group, and a wide range of educational experiences in the College Graduate group. Because school-based testing for learning disabilities was uncommon in the current generation of older Americans, we could not collect valid data on the incidence of learning disabilities in our cohort. However, anecdotal reports from our participants suggest that the No HS Diploma group included many participants with self-reported learning difficulties who struggled in high school, as well as those who were highly capable of completing high school but left before graduation for other reasons, such as military service or to work and support their families. We would expect to see very different neuropsychological test scores, as well as WRAT scores, from these types of participants, even though their level of educational attainment is identical. The College Graduate group included those with bachelor's degrees alone, as well as those with doctorates. WRAT estimates of ability may be especially likely to diverge from educational attainment for participants with very low or high levels of education.

The mere presence of discordance does not imply the superiority of one method over the other. To evaluate the WRAT and education-based methods, it is necessary to examine how accurately they identified those with true MCI: MCI which was more likely to progress to dementia. Results from the incident dementia analyses suggest an advantage for the WRAT method. Using the WRAT method, the HRs were either slightly or substantially higher than the HRs using the education method, for all five tests. The advantage of the WRAT was strongest for MCI-Ab: participants identified using the WRAT had a significantly increased risk of developing dementia, but those identified based on education did not. The WRAT advantage continued to hold when five levels of education were created by dividing the highly educated into two groups, suggesting that the advantage cannot merely be attributed to differences in years of education amongst college graduates (i.e. those with and without postgraduate education).

Our findings of considerable WRAT-score variability amongst participants with the same level of education help explain these results. It seems as though the premorbid abilities of the very low and very high scorers in each education level were not accurately captured by their years of education. For instance, the maximum WRAT score for the No HS Diploma group was a 54, which is the 75th percentile score for the College Graduate group. Our findings on discrepancies between WRAT and education echo those of other researchers,14,16,17 and they provide a rationale for why the WRAT is important. Each educational group consists of individuals with unique intellectual strengths, weaknesses, and experiences, both inside and outside the classroom, and this complexity cannot be conveyed by educational attainment alone. The Similarities test, a subtest of the WAIS, requires participants to think abstractly and then articulate a concise response. It is a high g-loading test: it is strongly correlated with other measures of intellectual ability.33 It may be especially responsive to premorbid abilities and educational experience. Educational exposure boosts one's performance on the test, as the abstract, scientific thinking required for high scores may be best learned in academic environments.33 It follows that, if the WRAT more accurately captures participants' premorbid abilities and educational quality, using the WRAT would help identify appropriate cut-off scores for this test.

Using both methods, a high percentage of participants with MCI-level scores at baseline “improved” or “reverted to normal” over time: Their scores from the second assessment were better than 1.5 SDs below the reference-group mean. The percentages ranged from 42% to 87%, depending on the test and method. Practice effects may have contributed to the high percentage of participants whose scores improved from the first visit to the second, a shortcoming common to many longitudinal studies of cognition. These effects, however, are likely attenuated by the average interval of 6 years between assessments, and the progressive nature of MCI. They are unlikely to lead to incorrect classification of MCI as normal at the second assessment. For most participants, the best explanation for their “improvement” is that they were incorrectly classified as MCI at baseline. The underlying cause of their low initial test scores was probably not MCI, but rather another factor, such as anxiety, depression, low motivation, inattention, or a lower/higher level of premorbid functioning than their education or WRAT scores predicted. Other studies have found high rates of “impaired” test scores in cognitively intact elderly participants, particularly with non-memory tests, suggesting that neuropsychological tests may be oversensitive in the elderly.13,20,21,34,35

Similar percentages of participants “improved” using both the WRAT and education method, suggesting that neither was superior at minimizing the problem of false positives. It is worth noting that some participants classified as MCI did not attend the second assessment but were classified as demented at follow-up based on other sources of information, such as medical records or neurology exams; if these participants had attended the second assessment, the percentage that “improved” may have been smaller.

Weaknesses of this study include the lack of racial diversity, the high number of participants (42.8%) who did not complete the second neuropsychological assessment, and the high percentage of participants who “reverted to normal” at their second assessment. The latter percentage was much higher than has been found by most other MCI studies. It is important to note, however, that this study was designed to investigate a specific methodological issue. The subjective components of the MCI criteria proposed by Petersen et al1 were not implemented here. This study used a psychometric method based on impairment on a single test. Jak et al20 found the single-test method to be the least reliable method for defining MCI, especially for nonamnestic tests, as many participants classified this way scored in the normal range at follow-up. Loewenstein et al21 and Snitz et al13 also found that single-test impairment failed to distinguish between normal aging and substantial, progressive cognitive impairment. For studies aiming to maximize the specificity of their MCI diagnoses, we caution against using the single-test impairment method. Prior research has identified poor memory and executive function test scores as strong predictors of subsequent dementia,3638 and results from this study supported our hypothesis that they would be the best indicators of genuine MCI. Performance on the Hooper Visual Organization Test was the least effective at identifying MCI.

One criticism of the WRAT as a measure of premorbid functioning is that it underestimates the abilities of those with weak reading skills but strengths in areas like math or science. (This is especially true of those with dyslexia, who can have an overall profile of high intelligence with isolated deficits in reading-related abilities.39) This is a fair criticism, but word identification is one of few cognitive abilities that does not rapidly decline in MCI.2426 Because MCI often affects one's capacity to exercise skills such as visuospatial reasoning and complex psychomotor speed,40 many tests used to assess intelligence in young adults are ill-suited for use in the elderly.

The use of a community-based cohort is a strength of this study. Rather than relying upon published norms from other studies, we were able to use normative data from our own cohort. The prospective study design also allowed surveillance for incident dementia in an established dementia-free cohort. The mean follow-up of 6 years between the baseline assessment and final diagnosis of dementia status is equal to, or longer than, that of most MCI studies.5,912,3537

Our findings indicate that the WRAT may be a better surrogate marker of premorbid abilities to use for determining cut-off scores for MCI diagnoses when compared with the commonly-used measure of educational attainment. Rentz et al14 has shown the benefits of using a word identification test to help identify cognitive impairment amongst the highly intelligent; this study supports using the WRAT in a study sample with a wide range of intellectual abilities. The WRAT can help improve diagnostic accuracy while adding only a few minutes of test administration, and it is particularly useful in assessing participants who lack previous testing data. Our results suggest that the WRAT should be considered for use as a complement to the standard measure of years of education.

Acknowledgments

Study funding: This work (design and conduct of the study, collection and management of the data) was supported by the Framingham Heart Study's National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contract (N01-HC-25195) and by grants from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 NS17950) and from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG16495; AG08122).

Footnotes

Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.

1

The use of these terms is not meant to imply that MCI-M, MCI-EF, etc. represent distinct subtypes of MCI. Instead, these three terms are used for the sake of simplicity and clarity. MCI-M represents amnestic deficits. The others represent non-amnestic deficits.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of internal medicine. 2004 Sep;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment--beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004 Sep;256(3):240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Luck T, Luppa M, Briel S, et al. Incidence of mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders. 2010;29(2):164–175. doi: 10.1159/000272424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fisk JD, Merry HR, Rockwood K. Variations in case definition affect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2003 Nov 11;61(9):1179–1184. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000089238.07771.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Griffith HR, Netson KL, Harrell LE, et al. Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: diagnostic outcomes and clinical prediction over a two-year time period. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006 Mar;12(2):166–175. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Agron J, et al. Cognitive profiles in Alzheimer's disease and in mild cognitive impairment of different etiologies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21(5–6):309–315. doi: 10.1159/000091522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, DeKosky ST, et al. Prevalence and classification of mild cognitive impairment in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study: part 1. Arch Neurol. 2003 Oct;60(10):1385–1389. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.10.1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Manly JJ, Bell-McGinty S, Tang MX, et al. Implementing diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community. Arch Neurol. 2005 Nov;62(11):1739–1746. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.11.1739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Palmer K, Backman L, Winblad B, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008 Jul;16(7):603–611. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181753a64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rasquin SM, Lodder J, Visser PJ, et al. Predictive accuracy of MCI subtypes for Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: a 2-year follow-up study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19(2–3):113–119. doi: 10.1159/000082662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ravaglia G, Forti P, Maioli F, et al. Conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: predictive role of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular risk factors. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21(1):51–58. doi: 10.1159/000089515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rountree SD, Waring SC, Chan WC, et al. Importance of subtle amnestic and nonamnestic deficits in mild cognitive impairment: prognosis and conversion to dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24(6):476–482. doi: 10.1159/000110800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Snitz BE, Saxton J, Lopez OL, et al. Identifying mild cognitive impairment at baseline in the Ginkgo Evaluation of Memory (GEM) study. Aging Ment Health. 2009 Mar;13(2):171–182. doi: 10.1080/13607860802380656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rentz DM, Huh TJ, Faust RR, et al. Use of IQ-adjusted norms to predict progressive cognitive decline in highly intelligent older individuals. Neuropsychology. 2004 Jan;18(1):38–49. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sharp ES, Gatz M. Relationship between education and dementia: an updated systematic review. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders. 2011 Oct;25(4):289–304. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, et al. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002 Mar;8(3):341–348. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.O'Bryant SE, Schrimsher GW, O'Jile JR. Discrepancies between self-reported years of education and estimated reading level: potential implications for neuropsychologists. Appl Neuropsychol. 2005;12(1):5–11. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1201_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wilkinson G. The Wide Range Achievement Test: Manual. 3rd ed. Wide Range; Wilmington, DE: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grober E, Sliwinski M. Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 1991 Nov;13(6):933–949. doi: 10.1080/01688639108405109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, et al. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009 May;17(5):368–375. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Small BJ, et al. Stability of different subtypes of mild cognitive impairment among the elderly over a 2- to 3-year follow-up period. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27(5):418–423. doi: 10.1159/000211803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, et al. The Framingham Offspring Study. Design and preliminary data. Preventive medicine. 1975 Dec;4(4):518–525. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(75)90037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Au R, Seshadri S, Wolf PA, et al. New norms for a new generation: cognitive performance in the framingham offspring cohort. Exp Aging Res. 2004 Oct-Dec;30(4):333–358. doi: 10.1080/03610730490484380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Schmand B, Geerlings MI, Jonker C, et al. Reading ability as an estimator of premorbid intelligence: does it remain stable in emergent dementia? Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 1998 Feb;20(1):42–51. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.1.42.1485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Johnstone B, Wilhelm KL. The longitudinal stability of the WRAT-R Reading subtest: is it an appropriate estimate of premorbid intelligence? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS. 1996 Jul;2(4):282–285. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700001296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ashendorf L, Jefferson AL, Green RC, et al. Test-retest stability on the WRAT-3 reading subtest in geriatric cognitive evaluations. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009 Jul;31(5):605–610. doi: 10.1080/13803390802375557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Seshadri S, Wolf PA, Beiser A, et al. Lifetime risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. The impact of mortality on risk estimates in the Framingham Study. Neurology. 1997 Dec;49(6):1498–1504. doi: 10.1212/wnl.49.6.1498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology. 1984 Jul;34(7):939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Friedman NP, Miyake A, Corley RP, et al. Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2006 Feb;17(2):172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ardila A, Pineda D, Rosselli M. Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2000 Jan;15(1):31–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Arffa S. The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2007 Nov;22(8):969–978. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hashimoto R, Meguro K, Lee E, et al. Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: the Tajiri Project. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006 Aug;60(4):422–428. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Flynn JR, Weiss LG. American IQ Gains From 1932 to 2002:The WISC Subtests and Educational Progress. International Journal of Testing. 2007;7(2):209–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Teng E, Tingus KD, Lu PH, et al. Persistence of neuropsychological testing deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;28(2):168–178. doi: 10.1159/000235732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mitchell J, Arnold R, Dawson K, et al. Outcome in subgroups of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is highly predictable using a simple algorithm. J Neurol. 2009 Sep;256(9):1500–1509. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5152-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tabert MH, Manly JJ, Liu X, et al. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006 Aug;63(8):916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rozzini L, Chilovi BV, Conti M, et al. Conversion of amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment to dementia of Alzheimer type is independent to memory deterioration. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007 Dec;22(12):1217–1222. doi: 10.1002/gps.1816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Elias MF, Beiser A, Wolf PA, et al. The preclinical phase of alzheimer disease: A 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort. Archives of neurology. 2000 Jun;57(6):808–813. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.6.808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ferrer E, Shaywitz BA, Holahan JM, et al. Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychol Sci. 2010 Jan;21(1):93–101. doi: 10.1177/0956797609354084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Arnaiz E, Almkvist O. Neuropsychological features of mild cognitive impairment and preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Acta neurologica Scandinavica. Supplementum. 2003;179:34–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES